2020
DOI: 10.1016/j.techsoc.2019.101222
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Psychological factors influencing laypersons’ acceptance of climate engineering, climate change mitigation and business as usual scenarios

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 67 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The standard deviation for stratospheric aerosol injection stands out as the highest of any of the options, whereas albedo management via clouds, conversely, is the lowest. Given that SRM options were ranked against one another, and not against CDR options, it is not possible to identify a pattern where SRM options such as stratospheric aerosol injection tend to be slightly less preferable, which is typical for public perceptions (Braun et al 2018 ; Jobin and Siegrist 2020 ; Wright et al 2014 ; Carlisle et al 2020 ; Pidgeon et al 2012 ; Bellamy et al 2016 ; Merk et al 2019 ; Klaus et al 2020 ). At the same time, the greater heterogeneity of expert opinion, especially with regard to stratospheric aerosol injection, mirrors the tendency for public evaluations to vary depending on the amount of information provided, mode of discussion, and over time (Braun et al 2018 ; Carlisle et al 2020 ; Merk et al 2019 ).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The standard deviation for stratospheric aerosol injection stands out as the highest of any of the options, whereas albedo management via clouds, conversely, is the lowest. Given that SRM options were ranked against one another, and not against CDR options, it is not possible to identify a pattern where SRM options such as stratospheric aerosol injection tend to be slightly less preferable, which is typical for public perceptions (Braun et al 2018 ; Jobin and Siegrist 2020 ; Wright et al 2014 ; Carlisle et al 2020 ; Pidgeon et al 2012 ; Bellamy et al 2016 ; Merk et al 2019 ; Klaus et al 2020 ). At the same time, the greater heterogeneity of expert opinion, especially with regard to stratospheric aerosol injection, mirrors the tendency for public evaluations to vary depending on the amount of information provided, mode of discussion, and over time (Braun et al 2018 ; Carlisle et al 2020 ; Merk et al 2019 ).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The scale used a five‐point Likert scale (from ‘Applied completely’ to ‘Does not apply at all’). At the time of its development, the reliability was α = 0.653 and was measured at the time of this study as α = 0.687 (Klaus et al., 2020 ; Kovaleva, 2012 ); The General Self‐Efficacy Scale (GSE‐10) where participants self‐rate themselves against 10 questions which examine self‐efficacy, the general belief in an individual's ability to respond to difficult situations, obstacles and setbacks. The scale used a five‐point Likert scale (from ‘Exactly true’ to ‘Not at all true’).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The scale used a fivepoint Likert scale (from 'Applied completely' to 'Does not apply at all'). At the time of its development, the reliability was α = 0.653 and was measured at the time of this study as α = 0.687 (Klaus et al, 2020;Kovaleva, 2012);…”
Section: Instrumentsmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…We do this by directly comparing support for individual CDR and SRM technologies, along with verifying previously identi ed predictors, with support for promoting GE over institutional-level mitigation, i.e., moral hazard beliefs. While previous studies such as Klaus et al (2020) have compared separate groups' responses to a mitigation scenario and GE scenarios, we have yet to see a study that asks all respondents to directly compare their preferences for mitigation, geoengineering, and other climate actions. By analyzing responses to this comparative question, our study uniquely allows us to identify contexts in which moral hazard or reverse moral hazard may arise.…”
Section: A Moral Hazard Contingency Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%