2013
DOI: 10.1515/jall-2013-0001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Prosody of focus marking in Ewe

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Specifically, in tone languages, morpho-syntactically unmarked (in-situ) focus can be marked with the use of various prosodic means. The variation includes the modification of scaling relations such as higher pitch range on the focused constituent and compression of the F0 of the post-focal part (PFC), as in Mandarin Chinese [13], longer duration of the focused constituent, as in Ewe [14], boundary insertion or enhancement, as in Naxi [15], and even no prosodic marking is possible, as in Northern Sotho [16], Hausa [17], and Yucatec Maya ( [18], [19]). To the best of our knowledge, no categorical prosodic means marking different focus types for tone languages are reported.…”
Section: Prosody Of In-situ Focus In Tone Languagesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically, in tone languages, morpho-syntactically unmarked (in-situ) focus can be marked with the use of various prosodic means. The variation includes the modification of scaling relations such as higher pitch range on the focused constituent and compression of the F0 of the post-focal part (PFC), as in Mandarin Chinese [13], longer duration of the focused constituent, as in Ewe [14], boundary insertion or enhancement, as in Naxi [15], and even no prosodic marking is possible, as in Northern Sotho [16], Hausa [17], and Yucatec Maya ( [18], [19]). To the best of our knowledge, no categorical prosodic means marking different focus types for tone languages are reported.…”
Section: Prosody Of In-situ Focus In Tone Languagesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There are syntactic options such as cleft structures and scrambling of word order [6][7][8][9][10]; there may be relevant morphological markers [11][12][13]; and there are prosodic changes in phrasing or in the phonetic implementation of the focused element and the surrounding material [14][15][16][17][18][19][20][21][22][23][24].…”
Section: Introduction mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There are a considerable number of studies investigating the prosodic realization of focus cross linguistically, which also covers the prosody of given information and which might involve repeated words. However, these studies do not directly address repetition effects and commonly make use of question-answer sequences (i.e., manipulated discourse structures) to elicit different focus types (e.g., Burdin et al, 2015; Fiedler & Jannedy, 2013). Studies on repetition reduction, on the other hand, include either spontaneous speech from corpora or semi-spontaneous dialogues from experimental tasks in which referential structures are generally not manipulated.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%