2016
DOI: 10.1111/sode.12182
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Prosocial Tendencies among Chinese American Children in Immigrant Families: Links to Cultural and Socio‐demographic Factors and Psychological Adjustment

Abstract: The present study examined relations between prosocial tendencies (dispositional sympathy and prosocial behavior) and psychological adjustment using a multimethod and multi-informant approach in a socioeconomically diverse sample of first-and second-generation Chinese American children from immigrant families (N 5 238, M age 5 9.2 years). We tested the concurrent associations between: (a) children's dispositional sympathy (rated by parents, teachers, and children, and observed prosocial behavior), (b) psycholo… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
14
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

3
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 66 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 56 publications
1
14
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Previous literature yields debates around the association between poverty and empathy (Fabes et al, 1999; Folkman, Lazarus, Dunkel‐Schetter, DeLongis, & Gruen, 1986; Main, Zhou, Liew, & Lee, 2017). However, the perspectives of strain and family process consistently demonstrate that economic stress negatively affect adolescent empathy and prosocial development by providing more possibilities for the youth to experience hostility rather than caring in a harsh family environment (Carlo, Padilla‐Walker, & Day, 2011; Davis & Carlo, 2019).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous literature yields debates around the association between poverty and empathy (Fabes et al, 1999; Folkman, Lazarus, Dunkel‐Schetter, DeLongis, & Gruen, 1986; Main, Zhou, Liew, & Lee, 2017). However, the perspectives of strain and family process consistently demonstrate that economic stress negatively affect adolescent empathy and prosocial development by providing more possibilities for the youth to experience hostility rather than caring in a harsh family environment (Carlo, Padilla‐Walker, & Day, 2011; Davis & Carlo, 2019).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The sample consisted of 239 children (48.1% females, M age = 9.2 years, SD = 0.73, age range = 7.5–11.0), their parents, and teachers who participated in a two‐wave longitudinal study on socioemotional and academic development of Chinese American children from immigrant families in the San Francisco Bay Area (Chen et al, 2014; Main, Zhou, Liew, & Lee, 2017). The present paper used data from W2 (collected 1.5–2.5 years after W1) because conflict was not assessed at W1.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While behavioral measures circumvent self-presentation bias associated with self-report, there are limitations to their use with older children due to masking (i.e., concealing one’s emotions with the expression of another) or expressive suppression (i.e., concealing one’s emotions by inhibiting emotional expressions), and social desirability remains an issue with these methods (see Main, Zhou, Liew, & Lee, 2017; Zhou, Valiente, & Eisenberg, 2003). Sometimes behavioral measures are supplemented with physiological indices, including heart rate and skin conductance, hypothesized to reflect more affective, less cognitively mediated aspects of empathy (Eisenberg et al, 1996).…”
Section: Empathy In Infants and Childrenmentioning
confidence: 99%