1996
DOI: 10.1016/0378-2166(94)00075-5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Promises in French children: Comprehension and metapragmatic knowledge

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
16
0
1

Year Published

1999
1999
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
3
16
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This is in line with previous work on French-speaking children's understanding of promises and future-directed commissive speech acts (''I will do it"; Bernicot & Laval, 1996), and it supports theoretical work arguing that using the word ''promise" is not a necessary condition for an utterance to count as a verbal commitment (Searle, 1969).…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 87%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…This is in line with previous work on French-speaking children's understanding of promises and future-directed commissive speech acts (''I will do it"; Bernicot & Laval, 1996), and it supports theoretical work arguing that using the word ''promise" is not a necessary condition for an utterance to count as a verbal commitment (Searle, 1969).…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 87%
“…can fulfill the same function (Searle, 1969). In fact, developmental work has shown that 3-to 10-year-old French children view future-directed speech acts (''I will do it") and assertions with a predictive content (''It will be done") as promises (Bernicot & Laval, 1996). However, under some circumstances, such as when statements containing ''promise" and ''will" provide conflicting evidence (e.g., regarding the location of a hidden toy), older but not younger children trust ''promise" statements more than ''will" statements (Lyon & Evans, 2014).…”
Section: P Kanngiesser Et Al / Journal Of Experimental Child Psychomentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The processes observed here are comparable to those described by Ackerman (1983), Bemicot (1992) and Bemicot & Legros (1987) for the comprehension of requests by 3-and 4-year-olds, and by Bernicot & Laval (1996) for the comprehension of commissives by children aged 3, 6 and 10. These processes include the use of the context as the first criterion for interpreting the utterance and then, whenever that context is unclear or not very informative, the interpretation is based on the linguistic characteristics of the utterance.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…Children from age 5 may comprehend promises, whereas those at age 6 are able to correctly identify obligation even if the target action is not accomplished (Bernicot & Laval, 1996;Maas, 2008), and finally, at age 7, children become sensitive to the promisor's intention in responsibility assignment of broken promises (Maas & Abbeduto, 2001). First, power symmetry in the peer-child context facilitated identification of a superordinate violator as compared to mother-child conditional promises.…”
Section: Age Differencesmentioning
confidence: 99%