Our system is currently under heavy load due to increased usage. We're actively working on upgrades to improve performance. Thank you for your patience.
2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.05.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Production of temporal terms by 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old children

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
46
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

3
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 66 publications
(51 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
1
46
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, in the case of number, infants also begin acquisition with non-linguistic representations of objects and approximate number and quickly recognize that numerals form a class of words that contrast in meaning (Brooks, Audet, & Barner, 2012;Tare, Shatz, & Gilbertson, 2008;Wynn, 1992), despite taking years to learn what these meanings are (see Carey, 2009, for review). Similarly, relatively early in acquisition, children recognize that time words like minute, second, and hour form a lexical class, but take many years to acquire their individual meanings (Busby Grant & Suddendorf, 2011;Shatz et al, 2010). Finally, children produce words that describe emotions from early in development, and understand that they belong to a class of words that describe human sentiment, but nonetheless take years to master their adult-like meanings, and form many interim hypotheses along the way (Widen & Russell, 2003).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…For example, in the case of number, infants also begin acquisition with non-linguistic representations of objects and approximate number and quickly recognize that numerals form a class of words that contrast in meaning (Brooks, Audet, & Barner, 2012;Tare, Shatz, & Gilbertson, 2008;Wynn, 1992), despite taking years to learn what these meanings are (see Carey, 2009, for review). Similarly, relatively early in acquisition, children recognize that time words like minute, second, and hour form a lexical class, but take many years to acquire their individual meanings (Busby Grant & Suddendorf, 2011;Shatz et al, 2010). Finally, children produce words that describe emotions from early in development, and understand that they belong to a class of words that describe human sentiment, but nonetheless take years to master their adult-like meanings, and form many interim hypotheses along the way (Widen & Russell, 2003).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…And were asked: When you come back tomorrow, can you show me how you will play the video? We chose "tomorrow" as our future time point because of its increasing use during preschool years (Friedman, 1992;Grant & Suddendorf, 2011;Harner, 1975;Suddendorf, 2010). Characters again appeared in new locations not previously seen.…”
Section: Materials and Proceduresmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Young children, especially those aged four and under, have considerable trouble understanding and correctly using specific future-oriented terms (Harner, 1975, 1980; Busby Grant and Suddendorf, 2010, 2011). Therefore, informing them that their choice of an item will have an effect when they go to another room in “five minutes” or play a game “tomorrow” (e.g., Russell et al, 2010) would be futile for a large proportion of young children.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%