2017
DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12718
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Probability Learning: Changes in Behavior Across Time and Development

Abstract: Individuals track probabilities, such as associations between events in their environments, but less is known about the degree to which experience—within a learning session and over development—influences people’s use of incoming probabilistic information to guide behavior in real time. In two experiments, children (4–11 years) and adults searched for rewards hidden in locations with predetermined probabilities. In Experiment 1, children (n = 42) and adults (n = 32) changed strategies to maximize reward receip… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

4
19
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
4
19
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Mean accuracy across all memory items was 77.2%. This finding also parallels a recent study in which, despite children accurately indicating the best option following a probabilistic learning task, they were less likely than adults to maximize their choices toward that option (Plate et al, 2018). Together these findings suggest Systematic Exploration in Young Children 10 that children's choices are not motivated by achieving maximum reward to the extent that adults' are.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 86%
“…Mean accuracy across all memory items was 77.2%. This finding also parallels a recent study in which, despite children accurately indicating the best option following a probabilistic learning task, they were less likely than adults to maximize their choices toward that option (Plate et al, 2018). Together these findings suggest Systematic Exploration in Young Children 10 that children's choices are not motivated by achieving maximum reward to the extent that adults' are.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 86%
“…The task was originally developed by Plate and colleagues [ 27 ], who gave permission for its use and adaptation. The original task consisted of 8 rocks, 200 trials, and the largest proportion of gold coins under the same rock was 70%.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The original task consisted of 8 rocks, 200 trials, and the largest proportion of gold coins under the same rock was 70%. The number of trials was too high for 25% of the children between ages 4 and 11 and for these children the data could not be used [ 27 ]. Therefore, we shortened the task to 120 trials, decreased the number of rocks from 8 to 6, and increased the largest proportion of gold coins under the same rock from 70% to 75%.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The task was originally developed by Plate and colleagues [27], who gave permission for its use and adaptation. The original task consisted of eight rocks, 200 trials and the largest proportion of gold coins under the same rock was 70%.…”
Section: Task 1: Probability Learning Task: Finding Gold Coins (Pl Gomentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The original task consisted of eight rocks, 200 trials and the largest proportion of gold coins under the same rock was 70%. The number of trials was too high for 25% of the children between age 4 and 11 and for these children the data could not be used [27]. Therefore, we shortened the task to 120 trials, decreased the number of rocks from eight to six, and increased the largest proportion of gold coins under the same rock from 70% to 75%.…”
Section: Task 1: Probability Learning Task: Finding Gold Coins (Pl Gomentioning
confidence: 99%