2017
DOI: 10.1523/eneuro.0061-17.2017
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Proactive Control: Neural Oscillatory Correlates of Conflict Anticipation and Response Slowing

Abstract: Proactive control allows us to anticipate environmental changes and adjust behavioral strategy. In the laboratory, investigators have used a number of different behavioral paradigms, including the stop-signal task (SST), to examine the neural processes of proactive control. Previous functional MRI studies of the SST have demonstrated regional responses to conflict anticipation—the likelihood of a stop signal or P(stop) as estimated by a Bayesian model—and reaction time (RT) slowing and how these responses are … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
19
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 24 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 100 publications
4
19
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This lack of RT progression suggests that children with ADHD, unlike TD children, do not develop an expectation of the target stimulus (the Nogo stimulus). This could be related to impairments in the allocation of cognitive resources for proactive cognitive control (Braver, 2012;Chang et al, 2017). This lack of contextual control for inhibiting the motor response in the Nogo trials contributes to the higher error rate of the ADHD group in the Nogo trials, a finding that is consistent with previous evidence (Spronk et al, 2008;Henríquez-Henríquez et al, 2015).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This lack of RT progression suggests that children with ADHD, unlike TD children, do not develop an expectation of the target stimulus (the Nogo stimulus). This could be related to impairments in the allocation of cognitive resources for proactive cognitive control (Braver, 2012;Chang et al, 2017). This lack of contextual control for inhibiting the motor response in the Nogo trials contributes to the higher error rate of the ADHD group in the Nogo trials, a finding that is consistent with previous evidence (Spronk et al, 2008;Henríquez-Henríquez et al, 2015).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…Proactive cognitive control is the ability to modify our responses anticipating the necessity of cognitive control based on the context provided by recent events (Hu and Li, 2012;Koechlin, 2014;Chang et al, 2017;Ryman et al, 2018), as opposed to reactive cognitive control, which requires the engagement of control processes at the onset of challenging task demands (Braver et al, 2009;Braver, 2012). Thus, proactive cognitive control requires the integration of past events and experience to adjust current goal-directed actions (Donoso et al, 2014), modifying and improving the cognitive control required for upcoming conflictive stimuli.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Several articles have shown that theta bursts (typically lasting a few hundred milliseconds) are associated with conflict or with conditions requiring enhanced cognitive control (e.g., Harper, Malone, & Iacono, ; McDermott, Weisman, Proskovec, Heinrichs‐Graham, & Wilson, , Töllner et al, ). Although some authors (e.g., Töllner et al, ) have linked these increases in theta activity to conflict monitoring per se, others have linked the amplitude of this signal to task complexity (e.g., Cooper et al, —who also reported an increase in delta activity; Voytek et al, ), task switching (e.g., Cunillera et al, ), or in some cases to the anticipation of conflict (e.g, Chang, Ide, Li, Chen, & Li, ). In general, investigators (e.g., Cooper et al, ; Phillips, Vinck, Everling, & Womelsdorf, ; Voytek et al, ) have associated bursts of frontal theta with facilitation of the reprogramming of the information processing system for a specific task (see also Bonneford et al, 2017, for attribution of this role to even lower frequency activities, such as delta).…”
Section: Interactions Between Phasic Burst and Oscillatory Phenomenmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Slowing of RT is instead negatively associated with IFG and posterior delta-theta activity. The results suggest that stop-expectancy and response-inhibition are processed by distinct frontoparietal networks, in coordination with temporally distinguished theta contributions (Chang et al, 2017 ). The evidence supports earlier-discussed fMRI findings (Hu et al, 2015a , b ; Manza et al, 2016 ) in that proactive behavioral inhibition does not map onto a specific brain region, but, instead, results from the interaction between distributed frontoparietal MDC networks (Hampshire and Sharp, 2015 ).…”
Section: Focal and Distributed Patterns Of Neural Synchrony In Behavimentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Stop-expectancy can be quantified trial-by-trial as stop-occurrence-probability from a dynamic Bayesian model (Yu and Cohen, 2009 ) and behaviorally, it correlates with RT slowing to go-signals. The spectral correlates of stop-expectancy and RT-slowing seem to be inversely related across trials (Chang et al, 2017 ). Stop-anticipation is accompanied by a pronounced low-theta activity in the supramarginal gyrus (SMG) and anterior SMC preceding, but not after, the occurrence of the go-signal.…”
Section: Focal and Distributed Patterns Of Neural Synchrony In Behavimentioning
confidence: 99%