2016
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00190
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Priming Children’s Use of Intentions in Moral Judgement with Metacognitive Training

Abstract: Typically, adults give a primary role to the agent’s intention to harm when performing a moral judgment of accidental harm. By contrast, children often focus on outcomes, underestimating the actor’s mental states when judging someone for his action, and rely on what we suppose to be intuitive and emotional processes. The present study explored the processes involved in the development of the capacity to integrate agents’ intentions into their moral judgment of accidental harm in 5 to 8-year-old children. This … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 56 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Given the efficacy of similar changes in previous research (e.g., Bearison & Isaacs, 1975;Feldman et al, 1976;Nelson et al, 1980;Nummedal & Bass, 1976), this is surprising, and suggests that failure to understand or recall occurred even more frequently in the large majority of previous research because outcomes are usually more salient and recent than intentions. However, Gvozdic et al (2016) propose that much of children's outcomebased judgment occurs because of the related issue of failure to inhibit automatic responses to outcomes. This would explain the effectiveness of their metacognitive training approach relative to ours, and suggests that instructing children to "not focus too much" on consequences would have resulted in higher levels of intention-based judgment by children.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Given the efficacy of similar changes in previous research (e.g., Bearison & Isaacs, 1975;Feldman et al, 1976;Nelson et al, 1980;Nummedal & Bass, 1976), this is surprising, and suggests that failure to understand or recall occurred even more frequently in the large majority of previous research because outcomes are usually more salient and recent than intentions. However, Gvozdic et al (2016) propose that much of children's outcomebased judgment occurs because of the related issue of failure to inhibit automatic responses to outcomes. This would explain the effectiveness of their metacognitive training approach relative to ours, and suggests that instructing children to "not focus too much" on consequences would have resulted in higher levels of intention-based judgment by children.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Feldman, Klosson, Parsons, Rholes and Ruble (1976) and Nummedal and Bass (1976) found that children's judgments became more intention-based when they reversed the order of presentation from the usual intention then outcome, to outcome followed by intention. More recently, Gvozdic, Moutier, Dupoux and Buon (2016) reported that metacognitive training, and in particular the use of an executive alert (to "not focus too much" on the consequences), resulted in 5-8 year-old children making adult-like, primarily intention-based judgments. This finding supports a cognitive resources account because it indicates that children's outcomebased judgment arises not from an inability to understand and use intention information, but from failure to inhibit their focus on outcomes.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To this end, our review proposes that strategies dedicated at mitigating the negative effects of violence have great chances of succeeding by targeting psychological mechanisms related to Empathy, Theory of Mind and Inhibitory control abilities. Encouragingly, recent prosocial behavior interventions have shown effective results in improving these abilities and could be used to disrupt the negative effects of violence on these mechanisms and, consequently, to ameliorate moral decisions among individuals displaying the negative behavioral effects resulted from having been exposed to violence (Chiu Ming Lam et al, 2011; Gvozdic et al, 2016; Mesurado et al, 2018).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While Piaget was clear that children begin with outcome-based evaluations and only later consider others’ intentions in their moral evaluation, more recent research produced heterogeneous results. Whereas some researchers suggest that even school-aged children tend to give more weight to outcomes than to intentions ( Costanzo et al, 1973 ; Yuill, 1984 ; Zelazo et al, 1996 ; Helwig et al, 2001 ; Cushman et al, 2013 ; Gummerum and Chu, 2014 ), others found that when using simplified procedures (e.g., simpler vignettes) or controlling for confounding factors (e.g., the action of the well-intended and the ill-intended actors led to the same outcome), even 4- to 5-year-old (and in some work, even 3-year-old) children consider an agent’s intention ( Chandler et al, 1973 ; Nelson, 1980 ; Baird and Astington, 2004 ; Nobes et al, 2009 , 2016 ; Vaish et al, 2010 ; Killen et al, 2011 ; Gvozdic et al, 2016 ). A recent study ( Josephs et al, 2016 ) demonstrated that 4-year-old (and to some extent even 3-year-old) children take into account an agent’s intentionality (freedom of choice) and protested more when a moral transgression occurred under free conditions than if it occurred under constrained ones.…”
Section: Investigating Children’s Understanding Of the Moral Dimensiomentioning
confidence: 99%