2014
DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2014.887000
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Prime time news: The influence of primed positive and negative emotion on susceptibility to false memories

Abstract: We examined the relation between emotion and susceptibility to misinformation using a novel paradigm, the ambiguous stimuli affective priming (ASAP) paradigm. Participants (N = 88) viewed ambiguous neutral images primed either at encoding or retrieval to be interpreted as either highly positive or negative (or neutral/not primed). After viewing the images, they either were asked misleading or non-leading questions. Following a delay, memory accuracy for the original images was assessed. Results indicated that … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

1
9
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
1
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This pattern highlights the different roles that gist and verbatim memory play for different types of valence, which is apparently more gist-based for negative valence and more verbatim-based for positive valence, a general pattern than has also been noted by others in word list experiments (Brainerd et al, 2008; Gomes, Brainerd, & Stein, 2013). This pattern is also consistent with previous research demonstrating that whereas negative information promotes false memory, positive information may actually protect against it (e.g., Porter, Spencer, & Birt, 2003; Porter, ten Brinke, Riley, & Baker, 2014). …”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…This pattern highlights the different roles that gist and verbatim memory play for different types of valence, which is apparently more gist-based for negative valence and more verbatim-based for positive valence, a general pattern than has also been noted by others in word list experiments (Brainerd et al, 2008; Gomes, Brainerd, & Stein, 2013). This pattern is also consistent with previous research demonstrating that whereas negative information promotes false memory, positive information may actually protect against it (e.g., Porter, Spencer, & Birt, 2003; Porter, ten Brinke, Riley, & Baker, 2014). …”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…Porter, Ten Brinke, Riley, and Baker (2014) manipulated valence by presenting ambiguous pictures, which were then described to subjects using positively- or negatively-valenced terminology. For instance, a picture depicting passengers boarding an airplane was described as headed toward a sunny vacation spot or a dismal war zone.…”
Section: Review Of Data I Emotional Content and False Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Van Damme and Smets (2014) modified Porter, Ten Brinke, et al’s (2014) design by manipulating the arousal as well as the valence of study pictures. They administered both high- and low-arousal positive and negative pictures, and they also administered medium- and low-arousal neutral pictures.…”
Section: Review Of Data I Emotional Content and False Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…At both interviews, our results indicated that children were more likely to falsely recall the negative than the neutral event. Subsequently, studies using suggestion-based false memory paradigms or other paradigms have shown similar findings in children as well in adults (e.g., Howe, Candel, Otgaar, Malone, & Wimmer, 2010;Porter, ten Brinke, Riley, & Baker, 2014).…”
Section: Methods In False Memory 11mentioning
confidence: 79%