Our system is currently under heavy load due to increased usage. We're actively working on upgrades to improve performance. Thank you for your patience.
2013
DOI: 10.1007/s00442-013-2702-z
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Prey subsidy or predator cue? Direct and indirect effects of caged predators on aquatic consumers and resources

Abstract: The non-consumptive effects of predators on prey can affect prey phenotypes, potentially having important consequences for communities due to trait-mediated indirect interactions. Predicting non-consumptive effects and their impacts on communities can be difficult because predators can affect resources directly through nutrient cycling and indirectly by altering prey resource use, which can lead to complex interactions among resources and consumers. In this study we examined the effects of caged dragonfly pred… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

2
31
2

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 32 publications
(35 citation statements)
references
References 48 publications
2
31
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Therefore, we expect that risk assessment should be more evident in prey traits that match the types of predators faced by the prey, avoiding unnecessary defense costs at high density. Our results highlight that experiments that systematically manipulate prey conspecific density could be as effective as experiments that manipulate caged predators (Peacor andWerner 2001, Costa andVonesh 2013) or the concentration of predation cues (Fraker 2008) to generate a gradient of predation risk. The present study, therefore, has important implications for evaluating past studies and for the design of future experiments.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…Therefore, we expect that risk assessment should be more evident in prey traits that match the types of predators faced by the prey, avoiding unnecessary defense costs at high density. Our results highlight that experiments that systematically manipulate prey conspecific density could be as effective as experiments that manipulate caged predators (Peacor andWerner 2001, Costa andVonesh 2013) or the concentration of predation cues (Fraker 2008) to generate a gradient of predation risk. The present study, therefore, has important implications for evaluating past studies and for the design of future experiments.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…The most direct evidence for processing chain effects arises when detritus generated by predators produces effects on microbial growth in the absence of intervening bacteriovores (prey) (Costa and Vonesh 2013). Experiment I shows that bacterial abundance increased from day 7 to day 14 in the infusion-alone controls ( IA , Fig.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Second, predator cues may stimulate costly defensive traits (e.g., behavioral changes) in prey that indirectly impact lower trophic levels (trait mediated indirect interaction, or TMII ) (reviewed by Werner and Peacor 2003). Third, predator consumption of prey may produce additional detritus in the form of predator feces, excreted nitrogenous waste, and partially eaten prey, releasing nutrients that fuel the growth of the basal trophic level, a phenomenon characterized as ‘nutrient cycling’ in systems dominated by periphyton and phytoplankton (Costa and Vonesh 2013). This effect of predator feeding is likely in detritus-based systems, where bacteria are the basal trophic level, and it is better described as a “processing chain interaction” (PCI) (Heard 1995).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations