1998
DOI: 10.2307/3546920
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Prey Size Reverses the Outcome of Interference Interactions of Scavenger Ants

Abstract: Cerda, X., Retana, J. and Cros, S. 1998. Prey size reverses the outcome of interference interactions of scavenger ants. -Oikos 82: 99-110.In the guild of scavenger ants in the Mediterranean habitats studied, there was a separation of prey on the basis of size: small species took small food items, and large ants collected large items. Nevertheless, some species enlarged their range of prey sizes through social mechanisms: Aphaenogaster senilis through recruitment in relatively small retrieval groups and subsequ… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

1
39
0

Year Published

2003
2003
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 48 publications
(40 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
1
39
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It also allowed P. bergi, to enlarge its diet by exploiting larger resources. Similar consequences of this behavior were observed in Aphaenogaster senilis in open Mediterranean habitats, which enlarged its range of prey sizes through cooperative carrying (Cerdá et al, 1998). P. spininodis' workers, although similar in size to P. bergi's workers (head width: 0.9 and 0.7 mm, respectively; G. Pirk, unpubl.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 61%
“…It also allowed P. bergi, to enlarge its diet by exploiting larger resources. Similar consequences of this behavior were observed in Aphaenogaster senilis in open Mediterranean habitats, which enlarged its range of prey sizes through cooperative carrying (Cerdá et al, 1998). P. spininodis' workers, although similar in size to P. bergi's workers (head width: 0.9 and 0.7 mm, respectively; G. Pirk, unpubl.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 61%
“…Because aggressive interactions are potentially lengthy and lethal to both participants (e. g., De Vita, 1979), workers should be more likely to initiate fights when they have the most to gain by winning (Starks et al, 1998). Therefore, workers may exhibit particularly high levels of aggression when competing for food (e. g., Fellers, 1987;Cerdá et al, 1998;Holway, 1999) or when defending their nest from foreign ants (e. g., Hölldobler, 1976), and assays that mimic these ecological contexts may induce more aggression than assays that mimic casual encounters between non-nestmates (Starks et al, 1998).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It may result in ''superefficient'' groups, in which multiple ants can collectively carry a larger weight than would be manageable if divided amongst the individual workers (Sudd, 1965;Franks, 1986;Moffett, 1988;Deneubourg and Beshers, 1991;Franks et al, 1999Franks et al, , 2001Robson and Traniello, 1998). Cooperative transport can also help reduce interspecific interference (Cerdá et al, 1998;Deneubourg and Beshers, 1991).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%