1989
DOI: 10.2307/3565418
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Prey Defences and Predator Handling Behaviour: The Dangerous Prey Hypothesis

Abstract: JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org. . Wiley-Blackwell and Nordic Society Oikos are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Oikos. Forbes, L. S. 1989. Prey defences and predator handlin… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

1
33
0
3

Year Published

1991
1991
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 60 publications
(37 citation statements)
references
References 18 publications
(15 reference statements)
1
33
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Perhaps it is the risk of damage that explains why oystercatchers do not exclude small clams Donax serra from their diet, while kelp gulls Larus dominicanus, which open their prey by dropping them from a height of 6 to 10 m, take only large clams (Ward 1991). Many other studies have considered the role of predation costs and/or potential damage to the predator in determining its diet: energetic and time costs of foraging in harvester ants (Fewell 1988); tooth wear in wild reindeer (Skogland 1988); wear a.nd tear of mouthparts in stream animals feeding on epilithic algae (Arens 1990); tooth breakage among large predatory mammals (Van Valkenburgh 1988); prey selection by molluscivorous fishes (Stein et al 1984, Slootweg 1987; risk of injury ('the dangerous prey hypothesis') in birds feeding on spiny catfish (Forbes 1989); and, prey selection in 2 neotropical hover-gleaning bi.rds (Sherry & McDade 1982).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Perhaps it is the risk of damage that explains why oystercatchers do not exclude small clams Donax serra from their diet, while kelp gulls Larus dominicanus, which open their prey by dropping them from a height of 6 to 10 m, take only large clams (Ward 1991). Many other studies have considered the role of predation costs and/or potential damage to the predator in determining its diet: energetic and time costs of foraging in harvester ants (Fewell 1988); tooth wear in wild reindeer (Skogland 1988); wear a.nd tear of mouthparts in stream animals feeding on epilithic algae (Arens 1990); tooth breakage among large predatory mammals (Van Valkenburgh 1988); prey selection by molluscivorous fishes (Stein et al 1984, Slootweg 1987; risk of injury ('the dangerous prey hypothesis') in birds feeding on spiny catfish (Forbes 1989); and, prey selection in 2 neotropical hover-gleaning bi.rds (Sherry & McDade 1982).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Several fish lineages have evolved spines that are formed from modified fin rays within one or more of their fins (dorsal, anal, pectoral or pelvic fin). Spines act as a defence against predators [17] by deterring predation attempts [18], reducing capture success or making it difficult for the predator to ingest the fish [19]. In wild fish populations, spine length increases with predator abundance [20,21] and predators have greater success eating individuals with shorter spines [22].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, small predators are expected to prefer relatively small prey because the time and/ or energy costs of handling and eating large prey are relatively great. Eating large prey may also involve greater risk of injury (Edmunds 1974, Forbes 1989, Shine and Webb 1993. In contrast, large predators are expected to prefer larger prey because the benefits of foraging for small prey are relatively meager (Griffiths 1980).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%