2011
DOI: 10.1080/10888438.2010.524463
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Prevalence and Reliability of Phonological, Surface, and Mixed Profiles in Dyslexia: A Review of Studies Conducted in Languages Varying in Orthographic Depth

Abstract: The influence of orthographic transparency on the prevalence of dyslexia subtypes was examined in a review of multiple-case studies conducted in languages differing in orthographic depth (English, French, and Spanish). Cross-language differences are found in the proportion of dissociated profiles as a function of the dependent variables (speed or accuracy), the classification method (classical vs. regressionbased methods), and the control sample (chronological age vs. reading level controls). The classical met… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
68
0
5

Year Published

2014
2014
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 94 publications
(76 citation statements)
references
References 66 publications
3
68
0
5
Order By: Relevance
“…This apparent mismatch is similar to a result found in dyslexic children (25%-39% of phonological profiles, Sprenger-Charolles et al, 2011). Such a discrepancy may indicate that most children with 2SLI have a deficit, albeit mild, in pseudoword reading.…”
Section: Comparisons Between Reading Disorder Subtypessupporting
confidence: 79%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This apparent mismatch is similar to a result found in dyslexic children (25%-39% of phonological profiles, Sprenger-Charolles et al, 2011). Such a discrepancy may indicate that most children with 2SLI have a deficit, albeit mild, in pseudoword reading.…”
Section: Comparisons Between Reading Disorder Subtypessupporting
confidence: 79%
“…The second method, multiple case analysis, fills this gap by identifying reading-disorder subtypes and their prevalence (Castles & Coltheart, 1993;Manis, Seidenberg, Doi, McBride-Chang, & Petersen, 1996;Sprenger-Charolles, Siegel, Jimé nez, & Ziegler, 2011). This has led to the identification of three reading profiles.…”
Section: Written Word Identification Assessmentmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Children with phonological dyslexia are very poor at reading pseudowords, but have relatively spared exception word reading, while children with surface dyslexia show the opposite pattern. Numerous studies across languages have documented that substantial percentages of children with dyslexia meet criteria for either phonological or surface dyslexia (Castles & Coltheart, 1993; Jimenez, Rodriguez, & Ramirez, 2009; Manis, Seidenberg, Doi, & McBride-Chang, 1996; Olson, Kliegl, Davidson, & Foltz, 1985; Sprenger-Charolles, Cole, Lacert, & Serniclaes, 2000; Sprenger-Charolles, Siegel, Jimenez, & Ziegler, 2011; Stanovich, Siegel, & Gottardo, 1997; Ziegler et al, 2008). However, very little research has investigated whether children identified as belonging to one subtype or another continue to exhibit that pattern over time.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The relationship between orthography and phonology is considered to vary as a continuum (e.g., Frost et al, 1987;Goswami, Gombert, & de Barrera, 1998;Seymour et al, 2003;Sprenger-Charolles, Siegel, Jiménez, & Ziegler, 2011). This implies that a given orthography can be classified along a single scale.…”
Section: Definitions To Date Existing Definitions Of Orthographic Depthmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Conversely, both approaches classified French as a predictable orthography − even compared to Dutch and German. In previous work on orthographic depth, French has often been described as an intermediate orthography (Goswami et al, 1998;Paulesu et al, 2001;Seymour et al, 2003;Sprenger-Charolles et al, 2011). The French orthography, therefore, shows the importance of distinguishing between the two concepts, as a failure to do so provides a different picture.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%