2020
DOI: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2020.102794
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Premasticated food transfer by wild chimpanzee mothers with their infants: Effects of maternal parity, infant age and sex, and food properties

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

0
5
1

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
0
5
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Our results augment the existing literature showing a lack of difference in maternal investment based on parity in terrestrial primates (cradle, restrain, retrieve: Seay, 1966; different spatial proximity measures: Silk, 1991). However, these findings contrast with studies on primates of varying degree of terrestriality in which multiparous mothers seem to invest more into (premasticated) food transfer (e.g., chimpanzee ( Pan troglodytes ): Bădescu et al, 2020) and independence promotion (e.g., rhesus macaques: Seay, 1966; Japanese macaques ( Macaca fuscata ): Tanaka, 1989) during the first six months of (offspring) age. With a larger sample size, follow up studies should investigate the interaction effect of mother’s parity and offspring age on maternal investment.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 81%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Our results augment the existing literature showing a lack of difference in maternal investment based on parity in terrestrial primates (cradle, restrain, retrieve: Seay, 1966; different spatial proximity measures: Silk, 1991). However, these findings contrast with studies on primates of varying degree of terrestriality in which multiparous mothers seem to invest more into (premasticated) food transfer (e.g., chimpanzee ( Pan troglodytes ): Bădescu et al, 2020) and independence promotion (e.g., rhesus macaques: Seay, 1966; Japanese macaques ( Macaca fuscata ): Tanaka, 1989) during the first six months of (offspring) age. With a larger sample size, follow up studies should investigate the interaction effect of mother’s parity and offspring age on maternal investment.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 81%
“…This was followed by an increase in the probability of mothers and offspring feeding in close proximity, peaking around 5-6 years of offspring age, which coincides with a steep increase in the immatures’ dietary breadth (Schuppli et al, 2021). Previous chimpanzee and orangutan studies also showed that mothers were increasingly less likely to share food items, as food processing competence of the offspring increases with age (e.g., Bădescu et al, 2020; Jaeggi et al, 2008; Mikeliban et al, 2021), indicating that mothers may be attuned to the developing competence of their offspring (Mikeliban et al, 2021). However, our current analysis cannot disentangle the role of mother vs. offspring in the probability of feeding in close proximity.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Second, foraging rates increased until chimpanzees were 1 year old, after which they remained stable, but foraging bout durations and % times continued to increase with age. This implies that by 1 year of age, infants were foraging as often as adult chimpanzees–which makes sense because chimpanzee infants foraged whenever their mothers did by this age [ 4 , 55 , 94 ]–but that they needed to forage longer per bout as they aged because they needed to supplement maternal milk with progressively more solid foods. Findings at other sites similarly showed that chimpanzee infants did not start feeding regularly on non-milk foods until close to 1 year of age, and that the overall time they spent foraging progressively increased through development [ 19 , 21 23 ].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Females at Ngogo occasionally share substantial amounts of premasticated food with their infants, and the food most often shared is also the most commonly eaten by the chimpanzees (a fig, Ficus mucuso ) [ 70 , 94 ]. We previously found that primiparous females shared premasticated food with their infants less often than multiparous females [ 94 ]. It may thus be that the infants of primiparous females needed to forage independently more to meet their dietary needs because their mothers shared less food with them.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Chimpanzees sometimes share meat from carcasses that they control 181,182 and food sharing is associated with elevation of oxytocin which may enhance bonding 183 . Plant foods are rarely shared among adult chimpanzees in the wild, but female chimpanzees regularly share plant foods with their offspring 185–187 . If the MRCA formed relatively stable polygynous breeding bonds, as the Ngogo chimpanzees do, sharing and exchange relationships might have been formed with bond partners, and represent a form of joint parental care.…”
Section: Implications For Understanding the Evolution Of Paternal Car...mentioning
confidence: 99%