2015
DOI: 10.15446/dyna.v82n193.53496
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Prediction of take-over time in highly automated driving by two psychometric tests

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…RTs are typically recorded from the start of a takeover request (Blommer et al, 2017;Dogan et al, 2017;Eriksson, Banks, & Stanton, 2017;Eriksson & Stanton, 2017a;Feldhütter et al, 2016;Gold, Damböck, Lorenz, & Bengler, 2013;Körber, Weißgerber, Kalb, Blaschke, & Farid, 2015;Lorenz, Kerschbaum, & Schumann, 2014;Melcher, Rauh, Diederichs, Widlroither, & Bauer, 2015;Naujoks et al, 2017;Naujoks, Mai, & Neukum, 2014;Naujoks, Purucker, Neukum, Wolter, & Steiger, 2015;Payre, Cestac, Dang, Vienne, & Delhomme, 2017;Petermeijer, Bazilinskyy, Bengler, & de Winter, 2017;Politis, Brewster, & Pollick, 2017;Radlmayr, Gold, Lorenz, Farid, & Bengler, 2014;Telpaz, Rhindress, Zelman, & Tsimhoni, 2015;van der Meulen, Janssen, & Kun, 2016;Vogelpohl et al, 2018;Wan & Wu, 2018;Zeeb, Buchner, & Schrauf, 2015) up to the point when the AV system becomes deactivated by the user (most commonly by the execution of a driving action: movement of the wheel or pedals, or a button press). Sometimes a takeover request is not present, so some studies identify RTs as starting from the moment that the event which precipitates the handover is initiated (e.g., a parked car becomes visible or a crosswind begins; Johns, Mok, Talamonti, Sibi, & Ju, 2017;Larsson, Kircher, Hultgren, & Andersson, 2014;Louw, Markkula, Boer, Madigan, Carsten, & Merat, 2017;Shen & Neyens, 2017;Strand et al, 2014).…”
Section: Reaction Times When Responding During Transitionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…RTs are typically recorded from the start of a takeover request (Blommer et al, 2017;Dogan et al, 2017;Eriksson, Banks, & Stanton, 2017;Eriksson & Stanton, 2017a;Feldhütter et al, 2016;Gold, Damböck, Lorenz, & Bengler, 2013;Körber, Weißgerber, Kalb, Blaschke, & Farid, 2015;Lorenz, Kerschbaum, & Schumann, 2014;Melcher, Rauh, Diederichs, Widlroither, & Bauer, 2015;Naujoks et al, 2017;Naujoks, Mai, & Neukum, 2014;Naujoks, Purucker, Neukum, Wolter, & Steiger, 2015;Payre, Cestac, Dang, Vienne, & Delhomme, 2017;Petermeijer, Bazilinskyy, Bengler, & de Winter, 2017;Politis, Brewster, & Pollick, 2017;Radlmayr, Gold, Lorenz, Farid, & Bengler, 2014;Telpaz, Rhindress, Zelman, & Tsimhoni, 2015;van der Meulen, Janssen, & Kun, 2016;Vogelpohl et al, 2018;Wan & Wu, 2018;Zeeb, Buchner, & Schrauf, 2015) up to the point when the AV system becomes deactivated by the user (most commonly by the execution of a driving action: movement of the wheel or pedals, or a button press). Sometimes a takeover request is not present, so some studies identify RTs as starting from the moment that the event which precipitates the handover is initiated (e.g., a parked car becomes visible or a crosswind begins; Johns, Mok, Talamonti, Sibi, & Ju, 2017;Larsson, Kircher, Hultgren, & Andersson, 2014;Louw, Markkula, Boer, Madigan, Carsten, & Merat, 2017;Shen & Neyens, 2017;Strand et al, 2014).…”
Section: Reaction Times When Responding During Transitionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, sub-second viewing times are probably too short for processing dynamic traffic scenes that require visual search by means of multiple fixations and saccades (see Rayner, 2009 for a review on eye movements and visual search). Lead times that are typically used in driving simulator research range between 2 s and 12 s (Gold et al, 2013;K€ orber et al, 2015Melcher et al, 2015;Mok et al, 2015;Samuel et al, 2016). In summary, our range of video lengths encompasses lead times that are commonly used, and range from extremely short (1 s) to longer than has been studied before (20 s).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…After taking over, the drivers were required to brake or steer to avoid a collision. Scenario 3 (critical) was similar to scenario 2, but the drivers' visual field was blocked by a large SUV ahead-not until the SUV changed lanes was the obstacle in front visible, and then the system issued an urgent TOR ( Figure 3) [38]. In all three scenarios, the takeover request lead time (TORLt) was defined as 7 seconds, which was set according to previous studies [6], [22].…”
Section: Experiments Designmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For critical level design of take-over scenarios, in most experiments, drivers avoid collisions by braking or changing lanes [6], [22], [23], [29], [35]- [37]. Some researchers even use a large vehicle to block obstacles in advance to increase the criticality of the scenario [38]- [40]. Although it is of utmost importance to know how quickly a driver can respond to a take-over request (TOR) and what the shortest TOR times are in emergencies, there is a paucity of research exploring the time it takes a driver to take-over control in normal, noncritical scenarios.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%