2016
DOI: 10.1037/xlm0000256
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Prediction in the processing of repair disfluencies: Evidence from the visual-world paradigm.

Abstract: Two visual-world eye-tracking experiments investigated the role of prediction in the processing of repair disfluencies (e.g., The chef reached for some salt uh I mean some ketchup…). Experiment 1 showed that listeners were more likely to fixate a critical distractor item (e.g., pepper) during the processing of repair disfluencies compared to the processing of coordination structures (e.g., …some salt and also some ketchup…). Experiment 2 replicated the findings of Experiment 1 for disfluency versus coordinatio… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
25
3

Year Published

2017
2017
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 23 publications
(33 citation statements)
references
References 62 publications
5
25
3
Order By: Relevance
“…However, this early commitment to a referential interpretation of eigenlijk did not result in faster dialogue completions: participants experienced sustained visual competition from alternative dialogue interpretations, and were slower to complete dialogues with the referent that contrasted with the contextually most likely interpretation. This finding is consistent with studies reporting lingering predictions or interpretations, as measured by sustained visual attention to initial interpretations that need to be revised as the linguistic input unfolds (e.g., Corley, 2010;Lowder & Ferreira, 2016). Moreover, it provides further support for the view that delays in processing of implicatures reflect additional processing costs associated with integrating the interpretation of a pragmatic inference with relevant information in the context, rather than the costs associated with computing a pragmatic inference itself (Grodner et al, 2010;Kurumada et al, 2014).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 87%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, this early commitment to a referential interpretation of eigenlijk did not result in faster dialogue completions: participants experienced sustained visual competition from alternative dialogue interpretations, and were slower to complete dialogues with the referent that contrasted with the contextually most likely interpretation. This finding is consistent with studies reporting lingering predictions or interpretations, as measured by sustained visual attention to initial interpretations that need to be revised as the linguistic input unfolds (e.g., Corley, 2010;Lowder & Ferreira, 2016). Moreover, it provides further support for the view that delays in processing of implicatures reflect additional processing costs associated with integrating the interpretation of a pragmatic inference with relevant information in the context, rather than the costs associated with computing a pragmatic inference itself (Grodner et al, 2010;Kurumada et al, 2014).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 87%
“…Linguistic elements that typically occur in conversational interaction and that have been studied experimentally are fillers, such as disfluencies (e.g., uh, uhm), repairs (e.g., oh, I mean) and backchannels (e.g., mhm, uhuh, really, oh). A range of psycholinguistic studies has shown that fillers can modulate listeners' expectations about, and/or facilitate processing of, upcoming input (e.g., Arnold, Hudson Kam, & Tanenhaus, 2007;Bosker, Quené, Sanders, & de Jong, 2014;Corley, MacGregor, & Donaldson, 2007;Corley, 2010;Fox Tree, 2001;Fox Tree & Schrock, 1999;Lowder & Ferreira, 2016). Various scholars argue that fillers also have a conversation-managing function.…”
Section: Discourse Markers and Discourse Processingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To the extent that initial predictions were not discarded, this is consistent with previous studies reporting lingering predictions or interpretations (e.g. Christianson, Hollingworth, Halliwell, & Ferreira, 2001;Corley, 2010;Lowder & Ferreira, 2016;Rommers & Federmeier, 2018b). Another possible explanation for the observed lack of modulation of the N400 by discourse markers is that the unpredictable words were selected from completions of dialogues containing eigenlijk.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 86%
“…visual world eye-tracking, e.g. Altmann and Kamide, 1999;Arai and Keller, 2013;Kaiser and Trueswell, 2004;Kamide et al, 2003;Kukona et al, 2011;Lowder and Ferreira, 2016). Note that people do not necessarily predict individual words but pre-activate information at various levels of representations (e.g.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%