2007
DOI: 10.1016/j.ijforecast.2007.05.006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Predicting Wimbledon 2005 tennis results by mere player name recognition

Abstract: The outcomes of matches in the 2005 Wimbledon Gentlemen's tennis competition were predicted by mere player name recognition. In a field study, amateur tennis players (n = 79) and laypeople (n = 105) indicated players' names they recognized, and predicted match outcomes. Predictions based on recognition rankings aggregated over all participants correctly predicted 70% of all matches. These recognition predictions were equal to or better than predictions based on official ATP rankings and the seedings of Wimbled… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
34
0
1

Year Published

2007
2007
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
2

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 92 publications
(38 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
1
34
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…For instance, Serwe & Frings (2006) reported that collective recognition of amateur players (who knew only half of the contestants) turned out to be a better predictor of the 2004 Wimbledon tennis match outcomes (72% correct) than did the Association of Tennis Professionals (ATP) Entry Ranking (66%), ATP Champions Race (68%), and the seeding of the Wimbledon experts (69%). Scheibehenne & Bröder (2007) found the same surprising result for Wimbledon 2006.…”
Section: Recognition Heuristicsupporting
confidence: 53%
“…For instance, Serwe & Frings (2006) reported that collective recognition of amateur players (who knew only half of the contestants) turned out to be a better predictor of the 2004 Wimbledon tennis match outcomes (72% correct) than did the Association of Tennis Professionals (ATP) Entry Ranking (66%), ATP Champions Race (68%), and the seeding of the Wimbledon experts (69%). Scheibehenne & Bröder (2007) found the same surprising result for Wimbledon 2006.…”
Section: Recognition Heuristicsupporting
confidence: 53%
“…If ␣ ϭ .5, recognition is not correlated with the criterion; if ␣ ϭ 1, recognition is perfectly correlated with the criterion (i.e., it always leads to a correct inference). Although recognition is a good cue in many real-world domains (e.g., cities' populations: Goldstein & Gigerenzer, 2002;Pohl, 2006; success in sports: Pachur & Biele, 2007;Scheibehenne & Bröder, 2007;Snook & Cullen, 2006;wealth: Frosch et al, 2007), it is not always. For instance, for predicting which of two French cities is larger, the recognition validity is .87 (on the basis of a sample of German participants; Goldstein & Gigerenzer, 2002); however, it is only .60 for predicting which of two infectious diseases occurs more frequently in Germany (Pachur & Hertwig, 2006).…”
Section: Adaptive Decision Making and Cognitive Agingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In general, RH use increases, the greater the importance of a quick decision (Hilbig, Erdfelder, & Pohl, 2012;Pachur & Hertwig, 2006) and the higher the validity of the recognition cue (Castela, Kellen, Erdfelder, & Hilbig, 2014;Hilbig, Erdfelder, & Pohl, 2010;Pachur, Mata, & Schooler, 2009;Pohl, 2006;Scheibehenne & Bröder, 2007). By contrast, integration of further knowledge increases as knowledge becomes more easily available and easier to integrate (Bröder & Eichler, 2006;Glöckner & Bröder, 2011;Hilbig, Michalkiewicz, Castela, Pohl, & Erdfelder, 2015; Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:10.3758/s13421-015-0567-6) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%