2017
DOI: 10.3758/s13415-017-0555-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Predicting risk decisions in a modified Balloon Analogue Risk Task: Conventional and single-trial ERP analyses

Abstract: Event-related potential (ERP) has the potential to reveal the temporal neurophysiological dynamics of risk decision-making, but this potential has not been fully explored in previous studies. When predicting risk decision with ERPs, most studies focus on between-trial analysis that reflects feedback learning, while within-trial analysis that could directly link option assessment with behavioral output has been largely ignored. Suitable task design is crucial for applying within-trial prediction. In this study,… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
38
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 40 publications
(39 citation statements)
references
References 122 publications
(161 reference statements)
1
38
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The relationships between decision-phase and outcomephase processes manifested in ERP components were examined in AUD and HC concerning the primary issue of whether abnormal risky decisions involve a failure at the decision phase (including processes such as reward prediction) and/or at the outcome phase (including prediction error and reward valuation), considering the modulation of these processes by impulsivity. A variant of the balloon analogue risk task 1 (BART; Lejuez et al, 2002Lejuez et al, , 2003, modified for EEG, was used for the assessment of risk-taking proneness (Lejuez et al, 2003) and particularly for distinguishing decision and feedback/outcome-related processes (Gu et al, 2018;Kiat et al, 2016). Its validity has been demonstrated in healthy individuals (Wallsten, Pleskac, & Lejuez, 2005) and in AUD (Fein & Chang, 2008) based on self-reports of risk behavior such as smoking, gambling, and unprotected sexual encounters (Lejuez et al, 2003) and based on relationships between sensation-seeking and/or impulsivity and decision behavior (Lauriola, Panno, Levin, & Lejuez, 2013;Lejuez et al, 2002).…”
Section: The Present Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…The relationships between decision-phase and outcomephase processes manifested in ERP components were examined in AUD and HC concerning the primary issue of whether abnormal risky decisions involve a failure at the decision phase (including processes such as reward prediction) and/or at the outcome phase (including prediction error and reward valuation), considering the modulation of these processes by impulsivity. A variant of the balloon analogue risk task 1 (BART; Lejuez et al, 2002Lejuez et al, , 2003, modified for EEG, was used for the assessment of risk-taking proneness (Lejuez et al, 2003) and particularly for distinguishing decision and feedback/outcome-related processes (Gu et al, 2018;Kiat et al, 2016). Its validity has been demonstrated in healthy individuals (Wallsten, Pleskac, & Lejuez, 2005) and in AUD (Fein & Chang, 2008) based on self-reports of risk behavior such as smoking, gambling, and unprotected sexual encounters (Lejuez et al, 2003) and based on relationships between sensation-seeking and/or impulsivity and decision behavior (Lauriola, Panno, Levin, & Lejuez, 2013;Lejuez et al, 2002).…”
Section: The Present Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This pattern of results identifies distinct anomalies at decision-and feedback-evaluation stages. Most fundamentally, the large decision P3 (Cuzen, Andrew, Thomas, Stein, & Fein, 2013;Lopez-Caneda et al, 2013) and its normal variation as a function of decision choice Gu et al, 2018;Steffen et al, 2011) argue that poor decision behavior in AUD is not a function of failing to engage decision prompts but is, at least in part, a subsequent failure to process differential feedback. The decision P3 (or N2-P3 complex) has been related to reward prediction (Hajcak et al, 2005;Steffen et al, 2011;Yeung & Sanfey, 2004) and to more effortful processing supported by the recruitment of additional neural or cognitive resources for difficult choices (Kiefer, Marzinzik, Weisbrod, Scherg, & Spitzer, 1998;Lopez-Caneda et al, 2012;Petit et al, 2014;Sirevaag, Kramer, Coles, & Donchin, 1989;Yee & Miller, 1994).…”
Section: T a B L E 4 Decision P3 Simple-effectsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…First, in most previous ERP studies, available options did not change throughout a whole experiment; thus, participants acquired knowledge on option characteristics before the time point of their presentation (Hajcak, Moser, Holroyd, & Simons, 2006;Zheng, Li, Wang, Wu, & Liu, 2015). As a result, it is challenging to ensure that the evoked electrophysiological signals are time-locked to option assessment (Carlson, Zayas, & Guthormsen, 2009;Gu, Zhang, Luo, Wang, & Broster, 2018). Second, individuals adjust their choices according to previous decision outcomes (O'Doherty, Cockburn, & Pauli, 2017;Schultz, 2004), which may in turn modulate the electrophysiological signals associated with option assessment in the next trial.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%