2011
DOI: 10.1007/s00221-011-2862-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Preceding movement effects on sequential aiming

Abstract: In this study, two experiments were devised to examine the control strategy used by individuals when performing sequential aiming movements. Of particular interest was the aiming behavior displayed when task difficulty was changed midway through a sequence of movements. In Experiment 1, target size was manipulated, as the targets were made either larger or smaller, between the 8th and 12th movement of the sequence. In Experiment 2, the amplitude between the two targets was similarly changed while the target si… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
2
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
1
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus, our group proposed that the observed SSR for antisaccades evinces that top-down control renders sensorimotor transformations via the same relative visual information as perceptions. 2 Moreover, we note that our conclusion is consistent with work demonstrating that the spatial location of a target on trial N-1 or N-2 influences the endpoint location for a to-be-performed trial (Rastgardani, Lau, Barton, & Abegg, 2012; see also Abegg, Rodriguez, Lee, & Barton, 2010;Cheng, De Grosbois, Smirl, Heath, & Binsted, 2011;DeSimone, Everling, & Heath, 2015).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 87%
“…Thus, our group proposed that the observed SSR for antisaccades evinces that top-down control renders sensorimotor transformations via the same relative visual information as perceptions. 2 Moreover, we note that our conclusion is consistent with work demonstrating that the spatial location of a target on trial N-1 or N-2 influences the endpoint location for a to-be-performed trial (Rastgardani, Lau, Barton, & Abegg, 2012; see also Abegg, Rodriguez, Lee, & Barton, 2010;Cheng, De Grosbois, Smirl, Heath, & Binsted, 2011;DeSimone, Everling, & Heath, 2015).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 87%
“…Participants may consider the instructions and the conditions they encountered within a session (and possibly previous sessions) to quickly develop a consistent movement pattern. Alternatively, they may change their behaviour after each tap on the basis of the circumstances during that tap, leading to serial dependencies across trials (Brenner and Smeets 2011 ; Cheng et al 2011 ; de Lussanet et al 2001 ; Volcic and Domini 2018 ). In both cases, one might expect participants to move faster in sessions in which there is never a choice between different kinds of targets.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Conversely, reducing task difficulty (via target width or distance manipulation) evokes an immediate lengthening of the deceleration phase followed by slower adjustments similar to those observed when increasing task difficulty. These findings were interpreted in terms of two concurrent processes at different time scales (Fernandez, Warren, & Bootsma, 2006; for similarities in a discrete Fitts's task, see Cheng, De Grosbois, Smirl, Heath, & Binsted, 2011;Heath, Hodges, Chua, & Elliott, 1998). The slow adaptation can be understood in terms of a parameterization of the functional dynamics (cf.…”
Section: Informational Sculpting Of Sfmmentioning
confidence: 99%