2022
DOI: 10.1016/j.pragma.2022.10.017
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Pragmatic competence without a language model: Other-Initiated Repair in Balinese homesign

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2023
2023
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In recent years, the body of work on other-initiated repair in different signed languages has been growing. The literature includes studies on Argentine Sign Language (LSA; Manrique 2016), Norwegian Sign Language (NTS; Skedsmo 2020a), Providence Island Sign Language (PISL; Omardeen 2021), cross-signing (situations in which two deaf people who do not share a language meet; Byun et al 2017) and Balinese homesign (Safar and de Vos 2022). In these languages, signers have been documented to use several means of open repair initiation, including question signs, mouthing of question words, and various nonmanual behaviors.…”
Section: Repair Initiatorsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In recent years, the body of work on other-initiated repair in different signed languages has been growing. The literature includes studies on Argentine Sign Language (LSA; Manrique 2016), Norwegian Sign Language (NTS; Skedsmo 2020a), Providence Island Sign Language (PISL; Omardeen 2021), cross-signing (situations in which two deaf people who do not share a language meet; Byun et al 2017) and Balinese homesign (Safar and de Vos 2022). In these languages, signers have been documented to use several means of open repair initiation, including question signs, mouthing of question words, and various nonmanual behaviors.…”
Section: Repair Initiatorsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, crosslinguistic research has shown that similar interactional environments can yield structurally similar words across unrelated languages (Dingemanse, Torreira, and Enfield 2013). In research on signed languages, too, there is growing interest in interactional phenomena such as backchanneling (e.g., Fenlon, Schembri, and Sutton-Spence 2013;Mesch 2016) and repair initiation (e.g., Byun et al 2017;Manrique 2016;Skedsmo 2020a;Safar and de Vos 2022;Omardeen 2021).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Highland diphthongal phones appear in highland areas such as Pupuan and Baturiti Districts, while lowland diphthongal phones appear in lower lands, such as Kediri and Selemadeg Timur Districts. The short duration of sound by highland people causes stress and tends to fall on the initial syllable of the word (Safar & de Vos, 2022;Wilson et al, 2014;Callan et al, 2004). Thus, it makes sense that the diphthongal phones in the highlands is formed at the beginning of the word and is not followed by other lower sounds.…”
Section: Geographical Factor (Non-linguistic Factor)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Lastly, in conversational settings, deaf homesigners will monitor their interlocutor's understanding of what is being expressed by responding to situations in which positive feedback by way of nodding is discontinued, or in which non-understanding is signaled through a puzzled facial expression by their interlocutor. Subsequent utterances are tailored to deal with the source of communicative trouble, supplementing their initial expression with additional forms to make themselves understood (Safar & de Vos, forthcoming). All in all, our observations of homesigners in their everyday social interactions indicate an ability to capitalize on semiotic resources to help build rapport with their interlocutors and re-establish mutual understanding when needed.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%