The platform will undergo maintenance on Sep 14 at about 7:45 AM EST and will be unavailable for approximately 2 hours.
2006
DOI: 10.1080/13546780500375663
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Pragmatic approach to decision making under uncertainty: The case of the disjunction effect

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

2007
2007
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 24 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 2 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Relevance Theory, by giving good predictions of these behaviors, instead revealed that people are not incoherent in the way they reason as they use contextual information in addition to the information explicitly provided to them, which makes it difficult to reason without a concrete context. The literature is quite abundant in this area and includes the logic of connectors (see for examples Politzer, 1986; Sperber et al, 1995; Noveck, 2001), Piaget's inclusion task (see for example Masson et al, 2016b; Politzer, 2016), bias in probability judgment (see for examples Hilton, 1995; Baratgin and Noveck, 2000; Baratgin and Politzer, 2006, 2007, 2010), and decision making (see for examples Bless et al, 1998; Bagassi and Macchi, 2006; Masson et al, 2016, 2017a).…”
Section: Theoretical Backgroundmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Relevance Theory, by giving good predictions of these behaviors, instead revealed that people are not incoherent in the way they reason as they use contextual information in addition to the information explicitly provided to them, which makes it difficult to reason without a concrete context. The literature is quite abundant in this area and includes the logic of connectors (see for examples Politzer, 1986; Sperber et al, 1995; Noveck, 2001), Piaget's inclusion task (see for example Masson et al, 2016b; Politzer, 2016), bias in probability judgment (see for examples Hilton, 1995; Baratgin and Noveck, 2000; Baratgin and Politzer, 2006, 2007, 2010), and decision making (see for examples Bless et al, 1998; Bagassi and Macchi, 2006; Masson et al, 2016, 2017a).…”
Section: Theoretical Backgroundmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This disjunction effect again produces a negative interference effect. However, only one study tried to replicate this and this study concluded that the results were produced by an artifact in the specific way the questions were asked and thus questioned whether the results actually test the sure thing principle (Bagassi and Macchi, 2006).…”
Section: Disjunction Effect In Decision Makingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There are many examples in the psychological literature that answers given, considered to be incorrect by the experimenter, by adult participants are actually the result of the participants' misunderstanding of the intentions of the experimenter. The utterances used and the context of the experimental task trigger implicatures in the participants that can induce answers that are different from those expected by the experimenter (see Dulany and Hilton, 1991;Sperber et al, 1995;Baratgin and Noveck, 2000;Macchi, 2000;Politzer and Macchi, 2000;Baratgin, 2002Baratgin, , 2009Bagassi and Macchi, 2006;Baratgin and Politzer, 2006, 2007, 2010Macchi and Bagassi, 2012;Macchi et al, 2019Macchi et al, , 2020. Many developmental studies also give pieces of evidence for the ability of children, given their age, to recognize the intentions of the communicator (see Braine and Shanks, 1965a,b;McGarrigle and Donaldson, 1974;Rose and Blank, 1974;Markman and Wachtel, 1988;Politzer, 1993Politzer, , 2004Politzer, , 2016Gelman and Bloom, 2000;Diesendruck and Markson, 2001;Bagassi et al, 2020, for examples).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%