Readings in Computer Vision 1987
DOI: 10.1016/b978-0-08-051581-6.50013-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Practical real-time imaging stereo matcher

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

0
23
0

Year Published

1992
1992
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
0
23
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, by introducing horizontal offsets between the random elements in some locations, the stimulus yields a percept of depth or depth structure when viewed stereoscopically. Importantly, these stimuli are remarkable because they demonstrate that disparity processing does not require monocularly identifiable elements or recognizable objects upon which to compute disparities; instead, the disparity extraction appears to be a low-level cross-correlation-like function, for which dense, random texture is, perhaps counterintuitively, actually the best input (Nishihara 1984). This implies an early extraction of disparities, likely at the point of initial binocular combination in primary visual cortex—as opposed to, say, after line segments are reconstructed in larger receptive fields or after a scene is parsed into meaningful elements.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, by introducing horizontal offsets between the random elements in some locations, the stimulus yields a percept of depth or depth structure when viewed stereoscopically. Importantly, these stimuli are remarkable because they demonstrate that disparity processing does not require monocularly identifiable elements or recognizable objects upon which to compute disparities; instead, the disparity extraction appears to be a low-level cross-correlation-like function, for which dense, random texture is, perhaps counterintuitively, actually the best input (Nishihara 1984). This implies an early extraction of disparities, likely at the point of initial binocular combination in primary visual cortex—as opposed to, say, after line segments are reconstructed in larger receptive fields or after a scene is parsed into meaningful elements.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In previous studies from this laboratory, relative response times were varied by either changing the spatial frequency of the stimuli (Gawne and Martin, 2002a; for references to the effects of spatial frequency on response latency see Marr and Poggio, 1979; Nishihara, 1984; Anderson and Van Essen, 1987; Parker et al, 1997; Bredfeldt and Ringach, 2002; Menz and Freeman, 2003; Frazor et al, 2004), or by changing the contrast and also the relative onset timing of the stimuli (Gawne, 2008). Evidence for rapid temporal gating was observed in these studies, where the temporal response to pairs of stimuli was completely dominated by the temporal response to the stimulus that by itself elicited the shortest latency.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Assessing the tuning of these channels has been of great importance for mechanistic models of stereo computer vision (Marr and Poggio, 1979; Nishihara, 1984; Quam, 1987; Rohaly and Wilson, 1993). These can be used to map different scales of matching in hierarchical structures (Nishihara, 1984; Quam, 1987) with, for instance, coarse-to-fine constraints (Rohaly and Wilson, 1993).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These can be used to map different scales of matching in hierarchical structures (Nishihara, 1984; Quam, 1987) with, for instance, coarse-to-fine constraints (Rohaly and Wilson, 1993). In robotic vision, these tuning properties can be used to calibrate cameras (Tsai, 1986) and vergence algorithms (Piater et al, 1999; Lonini et al, 2013).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%