1999
DOI: 10.1037/h0087309
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Polysemy effects in word naming.

Abstract: The present research examined the influence of polysemy on word naming. Naming was faster to polysemous than to nonpolysemous words. Moreover, polysemy interacted with word frequency such that the facilitative effects of polysemy were isolated to naming of low-frequency words. These findings are discussed with reference to the distributed models

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

4
21
0

Year Published

2002
2002
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(27 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
4
21
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In the past, effects have been reported for concreteness and imageability (e.g., Cortese et al, 1997;de Groot, 1989;James, 1975;Strain & Herdman, 1999;Strain et al, 1995;Zevin & Balota, 2000) and for polysemy (e.g., Borowsky & Masson, 1996;Gottlob et al, 1999;Hino & Lupker, 1996;Hino et al, 1998;Jastrzembski, 1981;Jastrzembski & Stanners, 1975;Kellas et al, 1988;Lichacz et al, 1999;Millis & Button, 1989;Pexman & Lupker, 1999;Rubenstein et al, 1970). The NOF effects reported here, presumably, are independent of these effects, because our word stimuli were all concrete nouns and were all nonpolysemous.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 49%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In the past, effects have been reported for concreteness and imageability (e.g., Cortese et al, 1997;de Groot, 1989;James, 1975;Strain & Herdman, 1999;Strain et al, 1995;Zevin & Balota, 2000) and for polysemy (e.g., Borowsky & Masson, 1996;Gottlob et al, 1999;Hino & Lupker, 1996;Hino et al, 1998;Jastrzembski, 1981;Jastrzembski & Stanners, 1975;Kellas et al, 1988;Lichacz et al, 1999;Millis & Button, 1989;Pexman & Lupker, 1999;Rubenstein et al, 1970). The NOF effects reported here, presumably, are independent of these effects, because our word stimuli were all concrete nouns and were all nonpolysemous.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 49%
“…That is, responding in those tasks is usually faster to polysemous words than to nonpolysemous words (Borowsky & Masson, 1996;Gottlob, Goldinger, Stone, & Van Orden, 1999;Hino & Lupker, 1996;Hino, Lupker, Sears, & Ogawa, 1998;Jastrzembski, 1981;Jastrzembski & Stanners, 1975;Kellas, Ferraro, & Simpson, 1988;Lichacz, Herdman, LeFevre, & Baird, 1999;Millis & Button, 1989;Pexman & Lupker, 1999;Rubenstein, Garfield, & Millikan, 1970). This effect has proven difficult to explain for current models of word recognition.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When the task is to read aloud as quickly and accurately as possible, the reading system uses whatever information is at its disposal for each word. In the case of highly imageable words, semantic information is particularly salient, and several kinds of semantic information, including imageability, have been shown to facilitate reading aloud (Strain et al, 1995; Hino and Lupker, 1996; Lichacz et al, 1999; Strain and Herdman, 1999; Hino et al, 2002; Shibahara et al, 2003; Balota et al, 2004; Rodd, 2004; Woollams, 2005; Yap et al, 2012). Here we have shown that such facilitation by semantics may be neurally instantiated as an area associated with semantics (ITS) exerting a causal influence on an area associated with phonology (pSTG).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Each group of nouns was evenly divided into low-frequency (LF) and high-frequency (HF) groups. Frequency was included as a factor due to previous research indicating thai frequency and lexical atnbiguity may interact in the process of lexical selection (see Hino & Lupker, 1996;Lichacz et al, 1999). and that older adulls show stronger frequency effects than younger adults (Spieler & Balota, 2000).…”
Section: Materials and Designmentioning
confidence: 99%