2004
DOI: 10.1038/nature02723
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Pinyon jays use transitive inference to predict social dominance

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
199
0
2

Year Published

2006
2006
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
7
3

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 340 publications
(213 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
7
199
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…The social intelligence hypothesis proposes that increased social complexity (frequently indexed by social group size) was the major selective pressure in primate cognitive evolution (6,44,48,50,87,115,120,(125)(126)(127)(128)(129)(130)(131)(132)(133)(134)(135)(136)(137)(138)(139)(140)(141). This hypothesis is supported by studies showing a positive correlation between a species' typical group size and the neocortex ratio (80, 81, 85-87, 129, 142-145), cognitive differences between closely related species with different group sizes (130,137,146,147), and evidence for cognitive convergence between highly social species (26,31,(148)(149)(150).…”
Section: Significancementioning
confidence: 99%
“…The social intelligence hypothesis proposes that increased social complexity (frequently indexed by social group size) was the major selective pressure in primate cognitive evolution (6,44,48,50,87,115,120,(125)(126)(127)(128)(129)(130)(131)(132)(133)(134)(135)(136)(137)(138)(139)(140)(141). This hypothesis is supported by studies showing a positive correlation between a species' typical group size and the neocortex ratio (80, 81, 85-87, 129, 142-145), cognitive differences between closely related species with different group sizes (130,137,146,147), and evidence for cognitive convergence between highly social species (26,31,(148)(149)(150).…”
Section: Significancementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Instead, an animal can learn its dominance status relative to a few animals whose rank is similar to its own and then, through observation, learn the dominance rank of the other animals, relative to those animals whose rank is already known. Recent research suggests that such dominance hierarchies can be acquired through observation by pinyon jays (Pas-y-Miño, Bond, Kamil, & Balda, 2004), hens (Hogue, Beaugrand, & Lague, 1996) and even fish (Grosenik, Clement, & Fernal, 2007). Although transitive inference may aid in the acquisition of dominance hierarchies, it seems unlikely that the efficient development of dominance hierarchies is responsible for the arbitrary form of transitive inference that has been found in nonsocial tasks.…”
Section: Transitive Inferencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…By observing interactions between the stranger and known individuals, with whom a social relationship has already been established, an animal may predict their own relationship to unknown individuals. This component of cognitive ability is called transitive inference (TI; Hogue et al, 1996;Paz-y-Miño et al, 2004;Engh et al, 2005;Grosenick et al, 2007;MacLean et al, 2008;Vasconcelos, 2008 for review). A variety of studies have suggested that transitive inference may be used by higher vertebrates (e.g., Hogue et al, 1996;Peake et al, 2002;Peake and McGregor, 2004;Engh et al, 2005), and TI studies are mainly documented in animals with high sociality, such as apes, monkeys, hyena, chickens and corvids (Gillan, 1981;Bond et al, 2003;Paz-y-Miño et al, 2004;Engh et al, 2005), as well as a territorial and social fish (Grosenick et al, 2007;White and Gowan, 2013).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%