1981
DOI: 10.2307/748927
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Piaget's Concept of Number Development: Its Relevance to Mathematics Learning

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
4
0

Year Published

1984
1984
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
1
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Findings tend to indicate, however, that exact sequences and concurrences may need to be reanalyzed and the operational model reassessed. For example, consistent with recent research, the acquisition of neither class inclusion nor conservation, whether through development or learning, appears to be an absolute prerequisite to the learning of many number skills (Fuson, 1982;Hiebert et al, 1982;Lemoyne & Favreau, 1981;Pennington et al, 1980;Saxe, 1979;Souviney, 1980). More importantly, the number skills treatment evidenced a greater transfer effect.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 64%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Findings tend to indicate, however, that exact sequences and concurrences may need to be reanalyzed and the operational model reassessed. For example, consistent with recent research, the acquisition of neither class inclusion nor conservation, whether through development or learning, appears to be an absolute prerequisite to the learning of many number skills (Fuson, 1982;Hiebert et al, 1982;Lemoyne & Favreau, 1981;Pennington et al, 1980;Saxe, 1979;Souviney, 1980). More importantly, the number skills treatment evidenced a greater transfer effect.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 64%
“…Attainment of at least some cognitive operations shows a moderate to high positive correlation with achievement in mathematics (Carpenter, 1980;Souviney, 1980). These operations do not appear to be absolute prerequisites for learning some number skills and concepts, although developmentally advanced children use more sophisticated strategies, and specific operations may be necessary to learn to apply more advanced processes (Fuson, 1982;Hiebert, Carpenter, & Moser, 1982;Lemoyne & Favreau, 1981;Saxe, 1979). However, correlational evidence affords a weak test of the premise.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These are often ignored by general theories. Thus, it is not a surprise that grasping Piagetian concepts, such as conservation of number, is not related to learning and doing arithmetic (Hiebert, Carpenter, & Moser, 1982;Lemoyne & Favreau, 1981;Pennington, Wallach, & Wallach, 1980). Also, these theories underestimated the complexity of development.…”
Section: Capitalizing On Developmental Theoriesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Piaget's seminal studies of children's numerical development have generated a large range of related studies. Gelman and Gallistel (1978: 3) for example, criticised Piaget's theory of number development as characterising children's knowledge in terms of what they could not do rather than what they could do, while others have investigated (e.g., Steffe, Hirstein & Spikes 1976;Lemoyne & Favreau 1981;Hiebert, Carpenter & Moser 1982) the relationships between Piagetian abilities such as quantitative comparisons and class inclusion, and children's learning of arithmetic topics such as addition and subtraction. Studies such as these have provided a foundation for the development of theories of number learning that are more content-specific (e.g., Steffe et al 1983;Steffe & Cobb 1988) than those that resulted from the pioneering work of Piaget.…”
Section: Radical Constructivism and Information Processing Wdifferentmentioning
confidence: 99%