1999
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.25.3.562
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

"Phonotactic influences on short-term memory": Correction.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

1
3
0

Year Published

2003
2003
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
1
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We argue, therefore, that our data are in line with the claim that verbal STM can be supported by general properties of the language system (e.g., Gathercole et al, 1999; as well as by specific characteristics of lexical representations (e.g., Gathercole et al, 2001;Romani et al, 2008;Roodenrys et al, 1994;Walker & Hulme, 1999). Such an influence of stored knowledge on verbal STM has been accounted for in a number of ways (as part of the working memory framework, Burgess & Hitch, 2005, 2006; in terms of redintegration, Hulme, Newton, Cowan, Stuart, & Brown, 1999;Hulme et al, 1997;Schweickert, 1993; and within the multiple-code hypothesis, Martin et al, 1999;Romani et al, 2008).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…We argue, therefore, that our data are in line with the claim that verbal STM can be supported by general properties of the language system (e.g., Gathercole et al, 1999; as well as by specific characteristics of lexical representations (e.g., Gathercole et al, 2001;Romani et al, 2008;Roodenrys et al, 1994;Walker & Hulme, 1999). Such an influence of stored knowledge on verbal STM has been accounted for in a number of ways (as part of the working memory framework, Burgess & Hitch, 2005, 2006; in terms of redintegration, Hulme, Newton, Cowan, Stuart, & Brown, 1999;Hulme et al, 1997;Schweickert, 1993; and within the multiple-code hypothesis, Martin et al, 1999;Romani et al, 2008).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…A number of studies have demonstrated that properties of stored lexical representations can support STM performance (e.g., Gathercole, Pickering, Hall, & Peaker, 2001;Roodenrys, Hulme, Alban, Ellis, & Brown, 1994;Walker & Hulme, 1999). Furthermore, more general properties of language (e.g., phonotactic frequency) have also been shown to benefit shortterm recall (Gathercole, Frankish, Pickering, & Peaker, 1999;Thorn, Gathercole, & Frankish, 2005).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is shown by the presence of a large number of psycholinguistic effects impacting the recall of item information, involving the different levels of language processing (phonological, lexical and semantic). At the sublexical, phonological level, it has been shown that verbal WM performance increases for nonwords containing high versus low phonotactic probability structures (Gathercole et al, 1999;Majerus et al, 2004Majerus et al, , 2012. Similarly, rhyming lists of items are characterized by less omission errors compared to non-rhyming lists (Fallon et al, 2005;Gupta et al, 2005).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…vWM closely interacts with phonological, lexical, and semantic linguistic variables. At the sublexical/phonological level, this is illustrated by studies showing that nonwords containing structures of high phonotactic probability (i.e., high biphone frequencies) are associated with higher vWM performance than nonwords containing structures of low phonotactic probabilities (Gathercole, Frankish, Pickering, & Peaker, 1999;Majerus et al, 2004). Likewise, the lexical levels of representation have also been shown to impact vWM performance, with higher recall performance for words than nonwords (Brener, 1940;Hulme, Maughan, & Brown, 1991;Jefferies, Frankish, & Lambon Ralph, 2006a), and high-frequency words also leading to higher recall performance as compared to low frequency words (Hulme et al, 1997;Hulme, Stuart, Brown, & Morin, 2003;Poirier & Saint-Aubin, 1996;Watkins & Watkins, 1977).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%