Speech Prosody 2014 2014
DOI: 10.21437/speechprosody.2014-73
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Phonetic variations : Impact of the communicative situation

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
11
2

Year Published

2014
2014
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
3
2

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 12 publications
6
11
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Some past research confirms this hypothesis [27][28]. Specifically, it was shown that sentences expressing controlling contexts were intoned using a louder voice, faster speech rate, higher pitch and harsher sounding voice quality than sentences expressing autonomy-supportive statements.…”
Section: Nonverbal Motivational Languagementioning
confidence: 86%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Some past research confirms this hypothesis [27][28]. Specifically, it was shown that sentences expressing controlling contexts were intoned using a louder voice, faster speech rate, higher pitch and harsher sounding voice quality than sentences expressing autonomy-supportive statements.…”
Section: Nonverbal Motivational Languagementioning
confidence: 86%
“…Thus, the current study explored prosody use in autonomysupportive and controlling contexts in Dutch parent-child interactions. Following previous research [27][28], it was expected that controlling and autonomy-supportive speech would be defined by distinct acoustic patterns as reflected in pitch, intensity and speech rate differences.…”
Section: Nonverbal Motivational Languagementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The latter set of data were particularly intriguing; even when reading motivationally neutral terms (e.g., "time to leave"), but with a motivational mindset that was either controlling or supportive, speakers expressed control with a low pitched, loud, and harsh voice using a fast speech rate, while autonomy-supportive prosody was conveyed using a high pitched, less loud voice while slowing down speech rate (Weinstein et al, 2014;. Similar results were observed in Dutch speaking parent-child interactions, where acoustic analyses supported previous results in a different language group and in naturalistic observations (Paulmann, Vrijders, Weinstein & Vansteenkiste, 2018).…”
Section: Cross-spliced Motivationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…studying for fun vs. for a grade) and it remains to be seen if comparable results are elicited when motivations are communicated through tone of voice. While research such as this has extensively explored which words may be used to communicate motivations (Hodgins, Brown, & Carver, 2007;Levesque & Pelletier, 2003;Radel, Sarrazin, & Pelletier, 2009;Ryan, 2012;Weinstein & Hodgins, 2009), relatively little is known about how the prosody, or tone of voice, used to deliver motivational messages (Weinstein, Zougkou & Paulmann, 2014;Weinstein et al, 2018, Zougkou, Weinstein, Paulmann, 2017, elicits differential responses from listeners.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%