2011
DOI: 10.1121/1.3621306
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Phonetic variability of stops and flaps in spontaneous and careful speech

Abstract: Variability is perhaps the most notable characteristic of speech, and it is particularly noticeable in spontaneous conversational speech. The current research examines how speakers realize the American English stops /p, k, b, g/ and flaps (ɾ from /t, d/), in casual conversation and in careful speech. Target consonants appear after stressed syllables (e.g., "lobby") or between unstressed syllables (e.g., "humanity"), in one of six segmental/word-boundary environments. This work documents the degree and types of… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

6
105
2
1

Year Published

2013
2013
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 90 publications
(116 citation statements)
references
References 33 publications
(37 reference statements)
6
105
2
1
Order By: Relevance
“…However, there is evidence that languages can resist articulatory pressures in their choice of surface realizations. Warner and Tucker (2011) report that the duration of American English /ɡ/ is marginally longer than that of /b/. Ohala (1983) argues that maintaining the voicing of /ɡ/ is more difficult than maintaining the voicing of /b/, which correlates with the higher likelihood of phonemic inventories not to have /ɡ/, compared with their likelihood not to have /b/ (Sherman 1975), and the frequency of the two segments in the world's languages (Maddieson 1984).…”
Section: What Determines Language-specific Duration and Deletion?mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…However, there is evidence that languages can resist articulatory pressures in their choice of surface realizations. Warner and Tucker (2011) report that the duration of American English /ɡ/ is marginally longer than that of /b/. Ohala (1983) argues that maintaining the voicing of /ɡ/ is more difficult than maintaining the voicing of /b/, which correlates with the higher likelihood of phonemic inventories not to have /ɡ/, compared with their likelihood not to have /b/ (Sherman 1975), and the frequency of the two segments in the world's languages (Maddieson 1984).…”
Section: What Determines Language-specific Duration and Deletion?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, Zue and Laferriere (1979) studied the distribution of word-medial /t, d/ surfacing as stops and flaps in several phonological environments in American English using word lists, each word embedded in a carrier phrase. Warner and Tucker (2011) compared the articulation of intervocalic oral stops in several phonetic environments and styles of speech, including read speech and phone conversations. Bouavichith and Davidson (2013) focused on intervocalic voiced stops in American English using words that were embedded in short stories.…”
Section: Different Approaches To the Study Of Segmental Propertiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…First, they occur frequently in both infant-directed and Ernestus, 2000) allemaal is reduced to [ɑməl] in 62.7 % of tokens and helemaal to [heməl] in 20.0% of tokens (Schuppler et al, 2011). Finally, as these words are neither function words nor typical content words, are hardly ever the topic of a conversation and do not refer to a physical entity, their pronunciation is not likely to be affected by predictability or redundancy.We compared infant-directed speech with both adult-directed informal speech and adult-directed read speech, as read speech generally involves more articulatory effort than spontaneous informal speech (Nakamura, Iwano & Furui, 2008;Strik, van Doremalen & Cucchiarini, 2008;Warner & Tucker, 2011). Therefore, if infant-directed speech is characterized by hyperarticulation and clarity, 10 we expect it to be similar to read speech with respect to phonetic reduction.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, many previous perception studies limited their scope to monosyllabic words ending in coronal stops (e.g., Port and Crawford 1989;van Rooy et al 2003;Warner et al 2004) and relied on small samples of speakers or stimulus items, such as testing 10 or fewer participants and/or stimulus items (e.g., Port and Crawford 1989;Kopkalli 1993;Matsui 2011;Röttger et al 2011). However, the patterns that exist at the level of individual speakers or specific words may not necessarily be representative of group-level behaviors and the entire lexicon, and factors such as word length and consonantal place of articulation are also well-known to play a role in the production of the voicing contrast in obstruents (among others, Gamkrelidze 1975;Ohala 1983;Warner and Tucker 2011). As such, the previously reported cases of the preservation of the voicing contrast in perception do not necessarily indicate that perceptual neutralization is incomplete in the language in general.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%