1983
DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1983.tb01849.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Phonemic segmentation and spelling

Abstract: Consciousness of the phoneme has often been studied in relation to reading skill. However, it is possible that reported differences in segmentation performance between good and poor readers are due to subjects' differences in spelling skill. Using two novel tasks which required the production of spoonerisms and the judgement of phoneme length respectively, segmentation in a group of subjects who read well while spelling poorly was compared with that of good readers/spellers and poor readers/spellers. Irrespect… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
84
1
9

Year Published

1991
1991
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
4
4
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 170 publications
(101 citation statements)
references
References 8 publications
6
84
1
9
Order By: Relevance
“…An example of such a task is reading span, in which the participant judges the semantic properties of sentences while remembering the last word of each sentence in sequence (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980, 1983. According to a recent proposal, verbal complex memory tasks such as these may also rely on the verbal storage component of working memory, the phonological loop (Baddeley & Logie, 1999;Duff & Logie, 2001).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…An example of such a task is reading span, in which the participant judges the semantic properties of sentences while remembering the last word of each sentence in sequence (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980, 1983. According to a recent proposal, verbal complex memory tasks such as these may also rely on the verbal storage component of working memory, the phonological loop (Baddeley & Logie, 1999;Duff & Logie, 2001).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Tests of phonological awareness vary considerably in terms of both the size of the phonological units to be manipulated and the degree of explicit metalinguistic awareness they require. Examples include judgments of rhyme (e.g., Bradley & Bryant, 1983), blending phonological elements (Mann & Liberman, 1984), deletion of phonological segments (e.g., Treiman & Baron, 1981), phoneme and syllable counting (e.g., Liberman, Shankweiler, Liberman, Fowler, & Fischer, 1977), judgments of shared phonemes in sequences of words (Kirtley, Bryant, MacLean, & Bradley, 1989), and spoonerizing pairs of verbal stimuli (Perin, 1983). Proficiency in phonological awareness tasks is highly associated with reading ability (e.g., Bradley & Bryant, 1985;Goswami & Bryant, 1990;, and has also been linked with vocabulary learning abilities (De Jong, Seveke, & van Veen, 2000;Metsala, 1999).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is demonstrated on tasks where one is required to repeat a word with a sound missing, such as, "Say sit without the "se"", or "Bind without the "ne"" (Stuart, 1990). For older participants the task might be to reverse the initial sounds of two words, such as "Chuck Berry" to give "Buck Cherry" (Perin, 1983). A problem characteristic of phonological dyslexics is inability to read nonsense words or new words that have to be constructed by translating letters into sounds (or graphemes into phonemes).…”
Section: Dyslexiamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the literature (e.g., MacLean, Bryant, & Bradley, 1987;Perin, 1983), the Brussels group is sometimes (at least more frequently than we had hoped) presented as defending the view that literacy acquisition is cause but not effect of segmental awareness. Although we have tried to clarify our position in recent papers (e.g., in Morais, Alegria, & Content, 1987a, 3987b), I have the uncomfortable feeling that some continue to misunderstand it.…”
Section: Segmentsmentioning
confidence: 99%