2016
DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1083593
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Perspective taking in referring to objectsbehindversusin front ofan observer: Frames of reference, intraindividual consistency, and response latencies

Abstract: Speakers of English and German typically adopt the reflection variant of the relative frame of reference (FoR) in order to describe how nonoriented objects that are located in front of them are related to one another. Little is known, however, about how they proceed in dorsal settings, with objects located in their back. In this article, we explore the turn hypothesis, which assumes a (mental) 180° turn of the observer to face the objects, converting the dorsal into a frontal situation, so that the preferred F… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
9
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

3
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
2
9
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Here, the descriptive results replicate the findings for the relative FoR variants obtained with other methods: a preference for reflection among German speakers (e.g., Grabowski and Miller, 2000; Beller et al, in press), mainly reflection, but also translation among English speakers (e.g., Hill, 1982; Grabowski and Miller, 2000), and a preference for translation among Tongan speakers (Bennardo, 2000). We therefore believe that our 2D sketches did not induce substantial biases.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 83%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Here, the descriptive results replicate the findings for the relative FoR variants obtained with other methods: a preference for reflection among German speakers (e.g., Grabowski and Miller, 2000; Beller et al, in press), mainly reflection, but also translation among English speakers (e.g., Hill, 1982; Grabowski and Miller, 2000), and a preference for translation among Tongan speakers (Bennardo, 2000). We therefore believe that our 2D sketches did not induce substantial biases.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 83%
“…Second, most of their responses can be interpreted as being consistent with the preferences we observed in the frontal settings, as argued in this section. And, finally, in a recent study with German participants we were able to replicate the results for both frontal and dorsal settings with perspectival photographs as stimuli (Beller et al, in press). …”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 57%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In addition, an observer with the same looking direction as that of the participant was inserted to emphasize perspective‐taking (cf. Beller, Bohlen, Hüther, & Bender, 2016; Beller et al, 2015). Apart from this, material and (overall) procedure were the same as in Study 1.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…More recent work on individual differences shows within-person stability of reference frames in peripersonal space (defined as the space immediately surrounding an individual; Rizzolatti, Fadiga, Fogassi, & Gallese, 1997). Using schematic diagrams of people and nonoriented objects (like squares and circles), Beller, Bohlen, Hüther, and Bender (2016) showed that German speakers generally prefer to reflect their frame of referencethat is, they adopt the orientation of a viewer in the scene, and report the position of objects relative to the view of the viewer. Moreover, measured across trials and over time, people are consistent, maintaining their reference frame preference in a simple schematic task.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%