2004
DOI: 10.1026/0044-3409.212.1.40
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Personenbezeichnungsmodelle auf dem Prüfstand

Abstract: Zusammenfassung. In zwei experimentellen Studien wurden anhand von längeren Texten die Thesen der Feministischen Linguistik überprüft, dass (1) das “generische“ Maskulinum zu einer “Benachteiligung“ von Frauen im Denken der SprachbenutzerInnen führt, was (2) durch sprachliche “Heilungs“varianten aufgehoben werden kann. In Studie 1 resultierte in einem sexuskonkretisierenden, Assoziationen an Männer auslösenden Kontext (Budapester Bäder) bei 220 Vpn sowohl für das “generische“ Maskulinum als auch für “Heilungs“… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
12
1
4

Year Published

2004
2004
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
7
3

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 32 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
0
12
1
4
Order By: Relevance
“…The gender-neutral usage of the masculine gender deviates from the more frequent relation of congruency between grammatical and biological gender for nouns that denote people (Duden Band 4, 1984, 1998. The question of whether masculine nouns in gender-neutral contexts are actually interpreted as gender neutral has been addressed in several experimental studies since the 1990s (Braun, Gottburgsen, Sczesny, & Stahlberg, 1998;Heise, 2000;Irmen & Kaczmarek, 1999;Irmen & Köhncke, 1996;Rothermund, 1998;Rothmund & Scheele, 2004;Scheele & Gauler, 1993;Stahlberg & Sczesny, 2001). Most studies' results agree that masculine generics lead to a lesser inclusion of women than men in mental representations compared to linguistic alternatives that comprise a feminine form.…”
Section: Findings From Studies On Masculine Generic Denotationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The gender-neutral usage of the masculine gender deviates from the more frequent relation of congruency between grammatical and biological gender for nouns that denote people (Duden Band 4, 1984, 1998. The question of whether masculine nouns in gender-neutral contexts are actually interpreted as gender neutral has been addressed in several experimental studies since the 1990s (Braun, Gottburgsen, Sczesny, & Stahlberg, 1998;Heise, 2000;Irmen & Kaczmarek, 1999;Irmen & Köhncke, 1996;Rothermund, 1998;Rothmund & Scheele, 2004;Scheele & Gauler, 1993;Stahlberg & Sczesny, 2001). Most studies' results agree that masculine generics lead to a lesser inclusion of women than men in mental representations compared to linguistic alternatives that comprise a feminine form.…”
Section: Findings From Studies On Masculine Generic Denotationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A variety of dependent variables were used to demonstrate that gender-fair language increases the mental representation of female exemplars compared to masculine generics, for example estimations of the gender distribution in certain groups (Braun et al, 1998), sentence finishing tests (e.g., Rothmund and Scheele, 2004), recognition tasks (e.g., Rothermund, 1998), or reading and reaction times (e.g., Irmen and Köncke, 1996; Irmen and Roßberg, 2004). The effects of gender-fair language seem to be moderated by participants’ attitudes toward gender-fair language (Stahlberg and Sczesny, 2001).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This language reform reflects the assumption that language, here gender-fair language, is a tool to influence people’s gendered perception of reality. For example, with respect to occupations, studies with adults and primary school children from countries with grammatical gender languages (e.g., French, German, Dutch, Spanish, Italian) suggest that they are perceived in a less gender-typed manner when they are described in gender-fair language, more specifically in pair forms (i.e., by explicit reference to both male and female jobholders, e.g., inventeuses et inventeurs ; French feminine and masculine plural forms for inventors), rather than masculine plural forms (e.g., inventors; Braun et al, 1998; Heise, 2000, 2003; Stahlberg and Sczesny, 2001; Stahlberg et al, 2001; Rothmund and Scheele, 2004; Vervecken et al, 2013; Vervecken and Hannover, 2015; for a discussion of this issue for natural gender languages e.g., English; see, e.g., Gabriel et al, 2008; Garnham et al, 2012; Lassonde and O’Brien, 2013). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%