2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2016.10.006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Personality and density distributions of behavior, emotions, and situations

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
65
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 86 publications
(69 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
4
65
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Research shows that people not only differ in their baseline, but also in the consistency of their trait‐relevant behaviours (Dalal et al , ). Moreover, individual differences in variability in trait‐relevant behaviour appear to be stable over time and can therefore be used to characterize individuals (Jones, Brown, Serfass, & Sherman, ), whereas personality variability has generally been found to be independent from the baseline, neuroticism is an exception to this rule as it is intrinsically linked with behavioural, cognitive and affective consistency by definition. That is, people who are generally low on neuroticism tend to show less variability in their affect, self‐esteem and neuroticism‐related behaviours (Eid & Diener, ; Fleeson & Gallagher, ; Kuppens, Oravecz & Tuerlinckx, ).…”
Section: The Elements Of the Personality Dynamics Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research shows that people not only differ in their baseline, but also in the consistency of their trait‐relevant behaviours (Dalal et al , ). Moreover, individual differences in variability in trait‐relevant behaviour appear to be stable over time and can therefore be used to characterize individuals (Jones, Brown, Serfass, & Sherman, ), whereas personality variability has generally been found to be independent from the baseline, neuroticism is an exception to this rule as it is intrinsically linked with behavioural, cognitive and affective consistency by definition. That is, people who are generally low on neuroticism tend to show less variability in their affect, self‐esteem and neuroticism‐related behaviours (Eid & Diener, ; Fleeson & Gallagher, ; Kuppens, Oravecz & Tuerlinckx, ).…”
Section: The Elements Of the Personality Dynamics Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…). Of course, it is possible to estimate any other person-specific parameter, such as weighted means, median, person-standard deviations (Jones, Brown, Serfass, & Sherman, 2017), or a number of other parameters (Dejonckheere et al, 2019;Zimmermann et al, 2019), each of which technically needs to be validated in their own right, and based on theoretical assumptions. The focus on average state scores in the current study reflects its popularity as an estimate of a person characteristic (see Table 1).…”
Section: Current Practicesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Note that the criterion must not necessarily be obtained at the same time as the average state score. The average state score is usually interpreted as a time-invariant characteristic of the person and therefore as stable (Fleeson, 2001;Jones et al, 2017). The average state score should therefore be related to other person characteristics, regardless of the time of their assessment.…”
Section: Average State Scoresmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Another venue is internal to traits. Personality researchers have also attempted to look at situations embedded in the traits (e.g., Church, Katigbak, & del Prado, 2010;Jones, Brown, Serfass, & Sherman, 2017;Murtha, Kanfer, & Ackerman, 1996;Ten Berge & De Raad, 2001, 2002Tett & Guterman, 2000;Yang et al, 2014). Either way, the connection between situations and traits can and should be examined in a more systematic manner.…”
Section: Inferring Traits From Situations Behaviors and Explanationsmentioning
confidence: 99%