2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.neunet.2011.05.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Persistent storage capability impairs decision making in a biophysical network model

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
30
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(31 citation statements)
references
References 79 publications
1
30
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Stronger non-selective input produced faster, less accurate decisions in the model. Furman and Wang (2008) did not show network activity under the different non-selective input rates, but it is clear from other modeling studies that the slope of network activity is higher (lower) with stronger (weaker) recurrent dynamics, corresponding to speed (accuracy) emphasis (e.g., Wong and Wang, 2006; Standage and Pare, 2011). Notably, the baseline rates of target-in and target-out movement neurons in the electrophysiological study by Heitz and Schall (2012) were higher (lower) under speed (accuracy) conditions, consistent with the modulation of local-circuit dynamics by a spatially non-selective signal.…”
Section: Three General Mechanistic Hypotheses On the Satmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Stronger non-selective input produced faster, less accurate decisions in the model. Furman and Wang (2008) did not show network activity under the different non-selective input rates, but it is clear from other modeling studies that the slope of network activity is higher (lower) with stronger (weaker) recurrent dynamics, corresponding to speed (accuracy) emphasis (e.g., Wong and Wang, 2006; Standage and Pare, 2011). Notably, the baseline rates of target-in and target-out movement neurons in the electrophysiological study by Heitz and Schall (2012) were higher (lower) under speed (accuracy) conditions, consistent with the modulation of local-circuit dynamics by a spatially non-selective signal.…”
Section: Three General Mechanistic Hypotheses On the Satmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Importantly, electrophysiological recordings from neurons responsive to a visual target that is not chosen on a given trial (target-out neurons) typically show a much lower rate of activity than target-in neurons prior to choice selection (e.g., Roitman and Shadlen, 2002; Thomas and Pare, 2007; Bollimunta and Ditterich, 2011; Ding and Gold, 2012). Taken together, increasing activity by target-in neurons and suppressed activity by target-out neurons have been interpreted as revealing competitive interactions between neural decision variables (Usher and McClelland, 2001; Wang, 2002; Albantakis and Deco, 2009; Standage and Pare, 2011). In competing accumulator models, each accumulator can be thought of as a population of neurons responsive to one of the alternatives, where the weight of subtraction corresponds to the strength of inhibition between these populations (Figure 2B).…”
Section: The Bounded Integration Frameworkmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…11B). A decaying function was used to mimic visual signals from visual area neurons (Standage and Paré 2011;Trappenberg et al 2001;Wong et al 2007). Figure 11, C and D, show the output responses of the accumulator units (x 1 and x 2 ).…”
Section: Computer Simulationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the simplest case, the decay of persistent activity could be fit with an exponential, and the value of this time constant of decay would explain the time constant of saturation in the dots-task responses. In relevant work from a visual search paradigm, NMDA receptors (which have a distinctively long time course) have been implicated in neural temporal integration (Shen et al, 2010; see also Standage and Paré (2011) for associated modeling). It is likely that cellular mechanisms such as NMDA receptors are critical within a recurrent network architecture (Wang, 2002).…”
Section: Implementation Issuesmentioning
confidence: 99%