2014
DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1308540110
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Persistence of social signatures in human communication

Abstract: The social network maintained by a focal individual, or ego, is intrinsically dynamic and typically exhibits some turnover in membership over time as personal circumstances change. However, the consequences of such changes on the distribution of an ego's network ties are not well understood. Here we use a unique 18-mo dataset that combines mobile phone calls and survey data to track changes in the ego networks and communication patterns of students making the transition from school to university or work. Our a… Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

26
365
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 307 publications
(392 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
26
365
1
Order By: Relevance
“…There is a trade-off between the number of social relationships (that is, N) and the mean strength of social relationships (that is, m) ( ( Hill and Dunbar, 2003;Roberts and Dunbar, 2011;Saramaki et al, 2014). Here, we found a simple law where N was inversely proportional to m a (a41).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 60%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…There is a trade-off between the number of social relationships (that is, N) and the mean strength of social relationships (that is, m) ( ( Hill and Dunbar, 2003;Roberts and Dunbar, 2011;Saramaki et al, 2014). Here, we found a simple law where N was inversely proportional to m a (a41).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 60%
“…Thus, we expect that they employ strategies to distribute the limited time resources to maximize benefits from their social relationships (Brown and Brown, 2006;Miritello et al, 2013b;Saramaki et al, 2014). As a result of such strategies, social relationship strengths (as measured by frequency of social grooming (Roberts and Dunbar, 2011;Arnaboldi et al, 2012;Song et al, 2012;Arnaboldi et al, 2013b;Fujihara and Miwa, 2014;Saramaki et al, 2014) may often show a much skewed distribution (Zhou et al, 2005;Arnaboldi et al, 2013b) (distributions following a power law (Hossmann et al, 2011;Arnaboldi et al, 2012;Hu et al, 2012;Song et al, 2012;Fujihara and Miwa, 2014)). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These numbers are smaller for @, e.g., 102,802 who and 31,477 whom, and even smaller for RE, with 27,227 who and 18,578 whom. In each case, whom is much lower than who, as expected because a small number of users tend to attract a large fraction of attention in both friendship [46,47] and online social [48][49][50][51][52] networks. This observation is confirmed in Figure 2, where we present Zipf plots associated to each interaction, clearly showing a strong heterogeneity in the system.…”
Section: Data Setmentioning
confidence: 78%
“…It would be particularly illuminating to observe the conditional frequencies with which individuals joined and left communities so as to test the hypothesis that joining rates are higher on the downside of each number and leaving rates higher on the upside. The hierarchically inclusive layered structuring of both natural communities and personal social networks is a consequence of a combination of cognitive limits on the number of relationships that can be maintained at a given emotional intensity and the time available to maintain such relationships (Miritello et al 2013;Dunbar 2011a, 2011b;Saramäki et al 2014;Sutcliffe et al 2012;Roberts et al 2014), with very characteristic frequencies of interaction for each layer that are, in fact, mirrored even in the online world (Arnabaldi et al 2015;Dunbar et al 2015). We might expect the camp communities to be organized in the same way.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%