1975
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1975.24-255
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

PERSISTENCE OF ACQUIRED BEHAVIORAL CONTROL IN THE CONTEXT OF IMPRINTING1

Abstract: Newly hatched Khaki Campbell ducklings (Anas platyrhynchos domesticus) were exposed to a moving object that immediately suppressed distress vocalizations occurring in a novel environment. The static visual and auditory features of this object acquired the ability to suppress distress vocalizations after eight 20-min sessions of exposure to the object in motion. The acquired suppressive properties of these features were found to persist throughout thirty 20-min sessions given over 10 days. During these sessions… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

1
12
0

Year Published

1977
1977
1992
1992

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

3
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 7 publications
1
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Among these similarities are the facts that both types of features are initially neutral in their effects upon ongoing distress vocalization (Eiserer & Hoffman, 1974;Hoffman et al, 1972), both types acquire suppressive properties only when they are paired with visual movement (Eiserer & Hoffman, 1974;Hoffman et al, 1972), and, after such acquisition, both types subsequently retain those suppressive properties throughout prolonged periods in which they are no Ionger accompanied by visual movement (Eiserer, Hoffman, & Klein, 1975). The present experiment adds still another Results Figure 2 shows the mean number of seconds of distress vocalization emitted during, and in the 10 sec immediately following, presentations of the moving object and its visual and auditory features, relative to parallel baseline periods.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Among these similarities are the facts that both types of features are initially neutral in their effects upon ongoing distress vocalization (Eiserer & Hoffman, 1974;Hoffman et al, 1972), both types acquire suppressive properties only when they are paired with visual movement (Eiserer & Hoffman, 1974;Hoffman et al, 1972), and, after such acquisition, both types subsequently retain those suppressive properties throughout prolonged periods in which they are no Ionger accompanied by visual movement (Eiserer, Hoffman, & Klein, 1975). The present experiment adds still another Results Figure 2 shows the mean number of seconds of distress vocalization emitted during, and in the 10 sec immediately following, presentations of the moving object and its visual and auditory features, relative to parallel baseline periods.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous work (Eiserer & Hoffman, 1973) has suggested that the same mechanisms underlie priming of both responses, since certain variables (e.g., prime duration and priming with the visual features) affect both behaviors in comparable ways. Measurement of the pecking response, however, offered an advantage over measurement of distress calls in that peking can be maintained over a large number of days (e.g., see Hoffman & Kozma, 1967), whereas ducklings frequently cease emitting baseline distress vocalization once they have habituated to the experimental apparatus-a process that may take but a few hours of exposure to the apparatus (Eiserer et al , 1975). Thus more trials were potentially possible with polepecking than with distress vocalization, and more trials would permit more sensitive assessment of any priming effects that might occur.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…It has been documented with several different types of imprinting objects (Eiserer, 1980;Hoffman et al, 1972); it applies to the initially neutral auditory as well as static visual features of an imprinting object (Eiserer & Hoffman, 1974;Hoffman et al, 1972); it can be assessed through a variety of filial behaviors, including suppression of distress calls (Hoffman et al, 1972), approach responses (Eiserer, 1980), operant responses (Eiserer et al, 1975), and-if Klopfer's (1965) findings are accepted as relevant-choice behavior; it can be seen in imprinting that occurs after as well as during the sensitive period (Gaioni et al, 1978); and, as indicated by the present work, it occurs in a wide variety of precocial birds. Given the empirical inconsistencies that often plague the imprinting literature and given the realization that seemingly important behavioral effects sometimes tum out to be highly dependent upon peculiar experimental parameters (Eiserer, 1978a), the contextual independence of the acquisition phenomenon is reassuring to those who would ponder its underlying processes.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As noted elsewhere (Eiserer, 1978b(Eiserer, , 1980, the acquisition of behavioral control by initially neutral features of an imprinting object seems to represent the very essence of infantile attachments to specific surrogate objects. Moreover, the acquisition phenomenon is of great theoretical interest as well, particularly in connection with classical conditioning vs. perceptual learning theories of imprinting (Eiserer, 1978b(Eiserer, , 1980 however, apparently the only precocial bird in which the effect has been specifically studied is the duck (Eiserer, 1977(Eiserer, , 1980Eiserer & Hoffman, 1973, 1974Eiserer, Hoffman, & Klein, 1975;Gaioni, Hoffman, DePaulo, & Stratton, 1978;Hoffman et al, 1972). Klopfer (1965) reported a similar effect in chicks; subjects exposed to a moving object subsequently preferred that object even when it was stationary.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%