2017
DOI: 10.1111/josl.12264
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Perceptual identification of talker ethnicity in Vancouver English

Abstract: Studies of ethnolinguistic variation typically begin by describing the speech production variables used to index social groups. In this study, we begin with indexical recognition – the perceptual identification of speakers’ self‐identified ethnic groups – to determine whether speakers produce ethnolinguistic variation and whether listeners are sensitive to it. Speech samples were recorded from thirty individuals from Metro Vancouver who self‐identified as Chinese, East Indian, or White Canadian. These utteranc… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
7
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
1
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…If we assume that ethnically Korean listeners have, overall, the most experience listening to Korean voices, that non-Asian listeners have the least experience, and that non-Korean Asian listeners fall somewhere in between, then we could conclude that greater experience hearing Korean American voices leads to greater accuracy in identifying Korean ethnicity in unfamiliar voices. These results accord with similar findings in Hanna (1997), Newman and Wu (2011), and Wong and Babel (2017). Although it is of course possible for a non-Korean listener to have extensive experience with Korean voices, or for a Korean listener not to have heard many Korean voices in their life (e.g., transnational Korean adoptees raised in a predominantly non-Korean neighborhood), we believe in the safety of this assumption: out of 105 listeners, 86 described themselves as something other than "Asian" for their own ethnic self-identification.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 93%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…If we assume that ethnically Korean listeners have, overall, the most experience listening to Korean voices, that non-Asian listeners have the least experience, and that non-Korean Asian listeners fall somewhere in between, then we could conclude that greater experience hearing Korean American voices leads to greater accuracy in identifying Korean ethnicity in unfamiliar voices. These results accord with similar findings in Hanna (1997), Newman and Wu (2011), and Wong and Babel (2017). Although it is of course possible for a non-Korean listener to have extensive experience with Korean voices, or for a Korean listener not to have heard many Korean voices in their life (e.g., transnational Korean adoptees raised in a predominantly non-Korean neighborhood), we believe in the safety of this assumption: out of 105 listeners, 86 described themselves as something other than "Asian" for their own ethnic self-identification.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 93%
“…Hanna (1997) hypothesized that a high-rising pitch contour throughout an utterance is characteristic of Asian American voices, while Newman and Wu (2011) analyzed several potential acoustic cues, including breathy phonation, Voice Onset Time (VOT), and formant quality of prevocalic /ô/, which all showed up in various combinations in the Asian American speech samples. Wong and Babel (2017) sampled Asian Canadian speakers, rather than Asian American speakers, but found some similar acoustic properties that were potentially attributable to a transfer effect from Cantonese into (North) American English, including a less-constricted postvocalic /ô/, /D/-stopping (e.g., this pronounced as dis), and less reduction in unstressed syllables.…”
Section: Asian American Identity and Speech Perceptionmentioning
confidence: 94%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The listeners who had not been made aware of the accent origins of the speakers did not display these speaker evaluation patterns, which also suggests that, in general, identifying specific accents is challenging to listeners. This is supported by research that shows that, beyond general distinctions between (non-)native accents, or a standard or non-standard accent, listeners often struggle to correctly identify the exact accent they are listening too, unless they are familiar with a particular accent (Gnevsheva 2018;Nolan 2003;Brunner 2009;McGorrery & McMahon 2017;Nejjari et al 2012;2020;Wong & Babel 2017).…”
Section: Non-native Evaluations Of Non-native Speakersmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…The Cantonese-speaking population in Canada is a diaspora that has extended roots (Yu, 2013), but the evidence as to whether the English of Cantonese-English bilinguals in Canada carries any unique signatures comparable to Hong Kong English is somewhat mixed. Controlled laboratory-elicited read sentences suggest pronunciation differences, if extant, are not perceptible to listeners (Babel & Russell, 2015), but more natural semi-spontaneous speech from early Cantonese-English bilinguals is categorized as associated with Chinese identity at levels well above chance (P. Wong & Babel, 2017). Hoffman and Walker (2010) examined the speech patterns of ethnically Chinese individuals in Toronto who were either first generation Cantonese-dominant speakers (n = 10) or second or third generation English-dominant speakers from historically Cantonese-speaking families (n = 23).…”
Section: Cantonese and Cantonese-english Bilingualismmentioning
confidence: 99%