2007
DOI: 10.1007/s10886-007-9291-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Perception of Conspecific Female Pheromone Stimulates Female Calling in an Arctiid Moth, Utetheisa ornatrix

Abstract: Perception of the female sex pheromone in Utetheisa ornatrix (Lepidoptera: Arctiidae) is responsible for induction and adjustment of calling by females and the collective phenomenon termed "female pheromonal chorusing". We found five olfactory-active compounds in the U. ornatrix female gland. When females were exposed to the entire pheromone or to two of its (synthetically prepared) components, (Z,Z,Z)-3,6,9-eicosatriene and (Z,Z,Z)-3,6,9-heneicosatriene, they were more likely to call during a given night, beg… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
11
0
1

Year Published

2007
2007
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
1
11
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This was not feasible for the present study because comparative data on release rates for moth species are very sparse. Both pheromone titre and release rates vary within species and within individuals, relating to age, time of day, and proximity of other individuals (e.g., Sanders and Lucuik 1972; Raina et al 1986; Foster et al 1995; Lim et al 2007). Although there is evidence that release rates are limited by pheromone gland titres (Schal et al 1987), and that pheromone titre is reflective of pheromone release rate (see Methods), it is possible that using fixed quantities of pheromone as a substitute for the quantity released is simply too inaccurate.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This was not feasible for the present study because comparative data on release rates for moth species are very sparse. Both pheromone titre and release rates vary within species and within individuals, relating to age, time of day, and proximity of other individuals (e.g., Sanders and Lucuik 1972; Raina et al 1986; Foster et al 1995; Lim et al 2007). Although there is evidence that release rates are limited by pheromone gland titres (Schal et al 1987), and that pheromone titre is reflective of pheromone release rate (see Methods), it is possible that using fixed quantities of pheromone as a substitute for the quantity released is simply too inaccurate.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Many female moths can perceive their own pheromone as well as pheromone compounds of other species (e.g., Ljungberg et al 1993; Schneider et al 1998; Groot et al 2005b; Hillier et al 2006). Also, most female moths produce their sex pheromone de novo every night (e.g., Raina 1989; Rafaeli 2002; Jurenka 2003), and can modulate the time and temporal patterning of pheromone release (calling) in relation to environmental conditions (Schal and Cardé 1985; Lim et al 2007) and presence of conspecifics (Conner et al 1980; Lim and Greenfield 2007, 2008). Hence, it may be possible that females can vary their biosynthesized as well as emitted pheromone blend to some extent depending on the prevailing olfactory cues in their habitat.…”
Section: Pheromone Composition Of Heliothis Virescens (Hv) Heliothismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Female calling behaviors are also modulated by sex pheromone reception5051, a form of autodetection that may mediate interaction among females to avoid competition. In general, mating disruption with synthetic chemicals may be costly, and its effect on female behavior may be limited52.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%