2008
DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.03.020
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Perceiving light versus material

Abstract: Humans rarely confuse variations in light intensity, such as shadows, shading, light sources and specular reflections, from variations in material properties, such as albedo or pigment. This review explores the cues, or regularities in the visual world that evidence suggests vision exploits to discriminate light from material. These cues include luminance relations, figural relations, 3D-shape, depth, colour, texture, and motion. On the basis of an examination of the cues together with the behavioural evidence… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

3
87
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 92 publications
(91 citation statements)
references
References 79 publications
3
87
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Although the importance of edge classification for lightness perception was realised a few decades ago (Gilchrist et al 1983), it has not been paid as much attention as it merits, perhaps because it has seemed that it would be possible to develop a theory of anchoring processes without resorting to this process (Gilchrist, 2006). Nevertheless, in the broader context of colour vision, a few illumination cues have been established (for a comprehensive review, see Kingdom, 2008). For instance, the straightness and fuzziness of a luminance edge were found to testify in favour of its being produced by an illumination edge; curvature, closedness, and sharpness, of its being produced by a material edge (Logvinenko, Adelson, Ross, & Somers, 2005).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Although the importance of edge classification for lightness perception was realised a few decades ago (Gilchrist et al 1983), it has not been paid as much attention as it merits, perhaps because it has seemed that it would be possible to develop a theory of anchoring processes without resorting to this process (Gilchrist, 2006). Nevertheless, in the broader context of colour vision, a few illumination cues have been established (for a comprehensive review, see Kingdom, 2008). For instance, the straightness and fuzziness of a luminance edge were found to testify in favour of its being produced by an illumination edge; curvature, closedness, and sharpness, of its being produced by a material edge (Logvinenko, Adelson, Ross, & Somers, 2005).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is vitally important from the ecological point of view to distinguish between these (e.g., Gibson, 1979). Human observers have been found to be rather good at distinguishing between changes produced by illumination and reflectance in visual scenes (e.g., Craven & Foster, 1992;Foster et al, 2001;Foster, 2003;Kingdom, 2008).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Finlayson et al's colour based method [9] defines an illumination-invariant colour space to discriminate shadows from reflectance variations, but this requires a calibrated camera. Further, since humans can distinguish shadows from reflectance changes in monochrome images [10] colour cannot be the only cue that enables such a separation.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, why should object recognition be at the core of shadow-related slowing? Here, the concept of combined heuristics for distinguishing "lighting" from "material" (Kingdom, 2008) may provide a framework within which to understand these effects. One might speculate that, in the upright shadow condition, such heuristics lead us to identify only the shadow casters (i.e., vertical bars) as "material" (i.e., objects), while the shadows (i.e., oblique bars) would be classified as lighting, not contributing to object recognition.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%