2015
DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1501592112
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Perceived intent motivates people to magnify observed harms

Abstract: Existing moral psychology research commonly explains certain phenomena in terms of a motivation to blame. However, this motivation is not measured directly, but rather is inferred from other measures, such as participants' judgments of an agent's blameworthiness. The present paper introduces new methods for assessing this theoretically important motivation, using tools drawn from animal-model research. We test these methods in the context of recent "harm-magnification" research, which shows that people often o… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
45
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 60 publications
(53 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
3
45
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This interaction is characterized by a superadditive relationship between the component factors. This is consistent with studies showing that intentionality augments the negative valence associated with the same harmful outcome (Gray and Wegner, 2008) and can even augment a person's quantification of the severity of a harmful outcome (Ames andFiske, 2013, 2015). Using functional imaging, we sought to parse how these two components, mental state and harm, converge into a punishment response that is defined by their interaction.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 89%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This interaction is characterized by a superadditive relationship between the component factors. This is consistent with studies showing that intentionality augments the negative valence associated with the same harmful outcome (Gray and Wegner, 2008) and can even augment a person's quantification of the severity of a harmful outcome (Ames andFiske, 2013, 2015). Using functional imaging, we sought to parse how these two components, mental state and harm, converge into a punishment response that is defined by their interaction.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 89%
“…This right DLPFC ROI also overlaps with the right DLPFC ROI previously hypothesized to be involved in the decision to punish (Buckholtz et al, 2008;. Previous studies investigating second-and third-party punishment decision-making have frequently found punishment decision-making to selectively engage the right as opposed to the left DLPFC (Sanfey et al, 2003;Knoch et al, 2006;Buckholtz et al, 2008;Baumgartner et al, 2014). Here punishment classification accuracy was similarly right-lateralized, as we failed to find any decoding (t ϭ 0.94, p ϭ 0.18 one-tailed) in a region with the same y and z coordinates in the left hemisphere.…”
Section: Fmri Datamentioning
confidence: 70%
“…Thus, when an observer believes that the agent did not intend to achieve an outcomerather, the outcome was unexpectedly achieved-the observer is likely to give less praise or blame to the agent for his or her actions, compared with when the observer believes the agent intended to cause the outcome (Krebs 1982). For example, damage caused unintentionally is underestimated and, therefore, a person who unintentionally causes damage is likely to receive less blame for their actions than a person who intentionally caused the same amount of damage (Ames and Fiske 2015).…”
Section: Intentionality and Product Preferencesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Mothers described in the vignettes were 10 years younger (ranging in age from [16][17][18][19][20][21][22][23] and held lower-paying jobs than in the standard vignettes (e.g., McDonalds cashier instead of accountant). Otherwise identical to Experiment 1.…”
Section: (N = 164)mentioning
confidence: 99%