APA Handbook of Sport and Exercise Psychology, Volume 2: Exercise Psychology (Vol. 2). 2019
DOI: 10.1037/0000124-009
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Perceived effort and exertion.

Abstract: People who move will all engage in perceived exertion (PE) to some degree. Perceptions of exertion accompany physical activity and play an important role in effort-regulation during physical work. Sensations perceived to be within the "comfort zone" will indicate no need to make any effort adjustments, whereas sensations perceived as too intense will inform a decision to adjust work output. CONCEPTUAL ISSUESDespite its ubiquity, the concept of PE is difficult to define. Perception of exertion involves a comple… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
16
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 129 publications
(187 reference statements)
0
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It has previously been shown that in order to maintain a constant level of exertion, individuals must continuously reduce force production, which typically exhibits a curvilinear pattern across time in men and women (Keller et al., 2021; Keller, Housh, Hill, et al., 2018; Keller et al., 2019a; Keller, Housh, Hill, et al., 2020). Hutchinson and Tenenbaum (2019) highlighted the significance of differentiating effort from exertion such that exertion has been related to sensations of pain and/or discomfort, whereas effort is a cognitive feeling of work associated with voluntary actions. It has been suggested that the perception of exertion is likely modulated by afferent feedback from the working muscles (i.e., group III/IV muscle afferents), subjective perceptions of force, inputs from a central motor command system, fatigue, and changes in motivation or task aversion (Hutchinson & Tenenbaum, 2019; Smirmaul, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…It has previously been shown that in order to maintain a constant level of exertion, individuals must continuously reduce force production, which typically exhibits a curvilinear pattern across time in men and women (Keller et al., 2021; Keller, Housh, Hill, et al., 2018; Keller et al., 2019a; Keller, Housh, Hill, et al., 2020). Hutchinson and Tenenbaum (2019) highlighted the significance of differentiating effort from exertion such that exertion has been related to sensations of pain and/or discomfort, whereas effort is a cognitive feeling of work associated with voluntary actions. It has been suggested that the perception of exertion is likely modulated by afferent feedback from the working muscles (i.e., group III/IV muscle afferents), subjective perceptions of force, inputs from a central motor command system, fatigue, and changes in motivation or task aversion (Hutchinson & Tenenbaum, 2019; Smirmaul, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Hutchinson and Tenenbaum (2019) highlighted the significance of differentiating effort from exertion such that exertion has been related to sensations of pain and/or discomfort, whereas effort is a cognitive feeling of work associated with voluntary actions. It has been suggested that the perception of exertion is likely modulated by afferent feedback from the working muscles (i.e., group III/IV muscle afferents), subjective perceptions of force, inputs from a central motor command system, fatigue, and changes in motivation or task aversion (Hutchinson & Tenenbaum, 2019; Smirmaul, 2012). Because these terms (i.e., effort vs. exertion) have different operational definitions and are associated with different fatigue-related symptoms, there is a rationale for theorizing different underlying mechanisms (perhaps not entirely exclusive of each other) that modulating their separate perceptions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…At higher intensities, however, neuromuscular and physiological factors override the influence of the psychological factors [38]. In addition, Hutchinson and Tenenbaum [39] suggested that during low intensity exercise, focus can easily shift between internal and external stimuli, but during high intensity or maximal exercise, focus cannot be voluntarily controlled " . .…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Therefore, the purpose of the present study was to examine the effect of anchor schemes on TTF, performance fatigability, neuromuscular responses, and the perceived sensations that contributed to task termination following sustained, isometric forearm flexion tasks. Based on the findings of previous studies [3,13,22,31,32,38], it was hypothesized that (1) TTF would be greater for the RPE fatigue task (RPEFT) versus the torque fatigue task (TRQFT) [13]; (2) there would be no differences between anchor schemes for the fatigue-induced changes in performance fatigability or neuromuscular responses [13]; (3) perceived fatigability of the muscles involved with the forearm flexion tasks would be greater for the TRQFT versus the RPEFT; and (4) the psychological factors related to perceived fatigability would have a greater level of contribution to the decision to terminate the task for the RPEFT than the TRQFT [38,39].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%