2014
DOI: 10.1177/0146167214523476
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

People Claim Objectivity After Knowingly Using Biased Strategies

Abstract: People tend not to recognize bias in their judgments. Such "bias blindness" persists, we show, even when people acknowledge that the judgmental strategies preceding their judgments are biased. In Experiment 1, participants took a test, received failure feedback, and then were led to assess the test's quality via an explicitly biased strategy (focusing on the test's weaknesses), an explicitly objective strategy, or a strategy of their choice. In Experiments 2 and 3, participants rated paintings using an explici… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
22
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
2
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 36 publications
(28 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
2
22
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, people learn others’ traits better after exposure to congruent stimuli (Carlston & Skowronski, 1994), suggesting a strong role of congruity in impression formation. One reason why these findings might seem counterintuitive is that people often claim objectivity in decisions despite their actual behavior (Hansen, Gerbasi, Todorov, Kruse, & Pronin, 2014). Gaining a richer understanding of congruity effects is therefore important.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, people learn others’ traits better after exposure to congruent stimuli (Carlston & Skowronski, 1994), suggesting a strong role of congruity in impression formation. One reason why these findings might seem counterintuitive is that people often claim objectivity in decisions despite their actual behavior (Hansen, Gerbasi, Todorov, Kruse, & Pronin, 2014). Gaining a richer understanding of congruity effects is therefore important.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Other questions surrounding information literacy or training include whether improvements persist over time or transfer domains, which, as noted, has not been found for critical thinking in general (e.g., Lilienfeld et al, 2009); whether a greater awareness of the problem, such as the prevalence of bots, leads to healthy skepticism or cynicism; and whether training efforts can "backfire," insofar as they do not improve the ability of individuals to assess information, but leave them feeling more confident in their biased judgments-as has been found in another context (Hansen, Gerbasi, Todorov, Kruse, & Pronin, 2014).…”
Section: So What's the Research Plan?mentioning
confidence: 91%
“…For instance, people typically judge that their beliefs reflect an objective and impartial assessment of the evidence (Ross & Ward, 1996). Even when people acknowledge that, in principle, they and others are likely to be biased (e.g., that people similar to them tend to be overly optimistic or let their identities affect their judgment), they often deny that they were influenced by these biases when asked about any particular judgment that they have made (Ehrlinger, Gilovich, & Ross, 2005;Hansen, Gerbasi, Todorov, Kruse, & Pronin, 2014;Pronin, Gilovich, & Ross, 2004;Pronin, Lin, & Ross, 2002;West, Meserve, & Stanovich, 2012). They also claim to prefer to hold beliefs that are logical and based on evidence, with a recent survey reporting that the majority of participants (78%) agreed with statements such as, "It is important to me personally that I can justify my beliefs using rational arguments and evidence" (Ståhl et al, 2016).…”
Section: The Case For Evidence-based Norms On Beliefmentioning
confidence: 99%