2016
DOI: 10.1075/ijchl.3.1.01yan
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Pattern substitution in Wuxi tone sandhi and its implication for phonological learning

Abstract: Tone sandhi in Wuxi Chinese involves “pattern substitution,” whereby the base tone on the first syllable is first substituted by another tone, then spread to the sandhi domain. We conducted a wug test to investigate native Wuxi speakers’ tacit knowledge of tone sandhi and found that the substituion aspect of the sandhi is not fully productive, but the extension aspect is, and sandhi productivity is influenced by the phonetic similarity between base and sandhi tones. These results are discussed in the context o… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 23 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It is noteworthy that although the Mandarin-speaking children have not reached the adult-like level, their tone sandhi application rate in pseudowords is still higher than adults' application rate in pseudowords where the sandhi items are operated lexically. For example, the sandhi application rate of adults in AG items was below 30% for Southern Min and Wuxi Wu [6] [29], but it was above 70% for Mandarin children aged 4 to 6. This may suggest that even though children adopt lexical mechanism at the early ages, they did not entirely rely on the lexical mechanism, for the productivity is apparently higher than the items that were purely operated via lexical mechanisms.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is noteworthy that although the Mandarin-speaking children have not reached the adult-like level, their tone sandhi application rate in pseudowords is still higher than adults' application rate in pseudowords where the sandhi items are operated lexically. For example, the sandhi application rate of adults in AG items was below 30% for Southern Min and Wuxi Wu [6] [29], but it was above 70% for Mandarin children aged 4 to 6. This may suggest that even though children adopt lexical mechanism at the early ages, they did not entirely rely on the lexical mechanism, for the productivity is apparently higher than the items that were purely operated via lexical mechanisms.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Take the Wuxi pattern as an example: the transparent spreading aspect of the sandhi can potentially be accounted for by the interaction between markedness constraints such as *TONE, *CONTOUR and faithfulness constraints such as MAX(Tone) and FAITH-ALIGN (which penalizes spreading, see [4]), but the opaque aspect of the sandhi needs to be encoded with listed sandhi tones on various levels of representation (e.g., syllable, morpheme, word) and constraints that force the listed forms to be used [49]. An implementation of this, which allows both the lexical patterns and the experimental results to be modeled, can be found in [42]. Other works that appeal to listedness to account for opaque tone sandhi patterns include [37] and [38].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Wuxi, therefore, has elements of both the Taiwanese and Shanghai types of sandhi. The productivity of the Wuxi tone sandhi pattern is tested in [42], and I summarize the results for T1-X, a cognate of the Shanghai 51-X pattern, here. Interested readers should consult [42] for more details.…”
Section: Wuximentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations