2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.bica.2013.05.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Pathfinding in the cognitive map: Network models of mechanisms for search and planning

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
5
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
1
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Bush et al (2015) reviews four more biologically plausible network models on Marr's implementation level. However, LIDA is concerned with the algorithmic level -and there is published behavioural evidence for such a mechanism (Mueller et al, 2013). We have previously succeeded in replicating two multi-goal route planning datasets using our simple model (in virtual as well as real environments -see (Madl, 2016)), which substantiates its cognitive plausibility.…”
Section: Neural Implausibilitiessupporting
confidence: 56%
“…Bush et al (2015) reviews four more biologically plausible network models on Marr's implementation level. However, LIDA is concerned with the algorithmic level -and there is published behavioural evidence for such a mechanism (Mueller et al, 2013). We have previously succeeded in replicating two multi-goal route planning datasets using our simple model (in virtual as well as real environments -see (Madl, 2016)), which substantiates its cognitive plausibility.…”
Section: Neural Implausibilitiessupporting
confidence: 56%
“…To deal with this, we implement a strategy to avoid revisiting failed clues, using counters shown in Figure 2 as Cycle and Attempt (that maps roughly onto a an activation marking past search; cf. Mueller et al, 2013 ). Although this particular implementation is somewhat ad hoc, the basic process is representative of a class of strategies that attempt to seek out novelty.…”
Section: An Recognition-primed Ai Model Of Crossword Playmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast, we observed that experts tended to make shorter, more deliberate moves from clue to clue, and appeared to solve clues that (1) were close to the current location in the puzzle, and (2) were already partially solved. We adapted a neurocomputational model of search goal selection (Mueller et al, 2013 ; Perelman and Mueller, 2013 ) to guide this Optimizing movement strategy model. The model first computes weights of each unsolved clue by using Equation (7).…”
Section: Model Simulationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In Experiment 1, we report two experiments conducted as part of a single project in which we evaluated the impact of mental stress induced by heat burden on a series of cognitive tasks whose summary values were reported by Mueller et al (2010) and Mueller et al (2013). The goal of these experiments was to establish how different cognitive functions were impacted by the physical and heat strain induced by wearing military protective gear (i.e., gas-impermeable clothing, gloves, goggles, and masks that enable the military to work in a potentially contaminated environment).…”
Section: Experiments 1a and 1bmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Along with serving as a useful exercise in optimization (e.g., Applegate, Bixby, Chvátal, & Cook, 2006), human-solved versions of the TSP (which permit approximate solutions) have become a method of growing interest for understanding visual problem solving, navigation, and planning (see MacGregor & Ormerod, 1996;MacGregor & Chu, 2011). colleagues (Mueller, Jones, Minnery, &Hiland, 2007, Mueller, 2008;2010) identified the problem as measuring an important aspect of biological intelligence, forming one part of a modern-day embodied Turing test, and have also connected it to naturalistic search and mental models of pathfinding (Mueller, Perelman, & Simpkins, 2013;Perelman & Mueller, 2013a;Perelman & Mueller, 2013b;Perelman, 2015;Perelman & Mueller, 2015).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%