2020
DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2019.102848
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Parts of me: Identity-relevance moderates self-prioritization

Abstract: Recent research has revealed a pervasive bias for self-relevant information during decision-making, a phenomenon termed the self-prioritization effect. Focusing almost exclusively on between-target (e.g., self vs. friend) differences in task performance, however, this work has overlooked the influence stimulus factors potentially exert during decisional processing. Accordingly, based on pertinent socialpsychological theorizing (i.e., Identity-Based Motivation Theory), here we explored the possibility that self… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

9
50
0
8

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

3
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 39 publications
(67 citation statements)
references
References 104 publications
(223 reference statements)
9
50
0
8
Order By: Relevance
“…Unlike Falbén et al, (2020), absent the operation of self-other response-related expectancies-and reflecting the strength of self-shape associations in working memorya self-prioritization effect was expected to emerge on matching trials under these conditions (Reuther & Chakravarthi, 2017;Sui et al, 2012;Wade & Vickery, 2017). 2 To identify the processes underlying task performance (i.e., stimulus and/or response biases), a drift diffusion model (DDM) analysis was conducted on the data as this approach has been applied successfully in previous work exploring the origins of stimulus prioritization (Falbén et al, 2020;Golubickis et al, 2017Golubickis et al, , 2018Golubickis et al, , 2020. Replicating previous research that has used shape-label matching tasks to probe self-bias (Golubickis et al, 2017(Golubickis et al, , 2020, we expected self-prioritization to be underpinned by differences in the rate of information uptake during decisional processing.…”
Section: The Current Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Unlike Falbén et al, (2020), absent the operation of self-other response-related expectancies-and reflecting the strength of self-shape associations in working memorya self-prioritization effect was expected to emerge on matching trials under these conditions (Reuther & Chakravarthi, 2017;Sui et al, 2012;Wade & Vickery, 2017). 2 To identify the processes underlying task performance (i.e., stimulus and/or response biases), a drift diffusion model (DDM) analysis was conducted on the data as this approach has been applied successfully in previous work exploring the origins of stimulus prioritization (Falbén et al, 2020;Golubickis et al, 2017Golubickis et al, , 2018Golubickis et al, , 2020. Replicating previous research that has used shape-label matching tasks to probe self-bias (Golubickis et al, 2017(Golubickis et al, , 2020, we expected self-prioritization to be underpinned by differences in the rate of information uptake during decisional processing.…”
Section: The Current Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is possible, however, that the rate at which priors are updated may vary as a function of target (i.e., self vs. other), reflecting the status of the self as a fundamental information-processing hub ( Humphreys & Sui, 2016 ; Sui & Humphreys, 2015 , 2017 ; Sui & Rotshtein, 2019 ). In addition, updating may be sensitive to specific properties of objects—including valence and value—that have implications for the self-concept (e.g., self-enhancement motivation; Golubickis, Ho et al, in press ; Sedikides & Gregg, 2008 ) or people’s evaluations of others. Using appropriate methodological/analytical techniques, a useful task for future research will be to explore these issues.…”
Section: The Origins Of Self-prioritisationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It remains to be seen whether the current effects would extend to paradigms in which the stimuli have greater meaning for people and self-object relations are created and assessed in different ways (e.g., perceptual-matching tasks, prior-entry effects, visual search). For example, it is possible that, in combination with a response bias, prioritisation effects may be underpinned by differences in the efficiency of visual processing (i.e., drift rates) when stimuli have particular significance for people or the difficulty of the task is increased ( Golubickis, Ho et al, in press ). In addition, although the current findings demonstrate that self-prioritisation can be eliminated by prior knowledge indicating the prevalence of to-be-judged stimuli, whether this is consistently the case remains an open question.…”
Section: The Origins Of Self-prioritisationmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…What is clear, at a psychological level, is that CEEing, distinct from visual preprocessing, is influenced by the activation of non-sensory representations including expectations, Bayesian priors, memories, associations, beliefs, stereotypes, schemas, and other bodily response representations when sensory inputs are ambiguous, absent, or incomplete (de Lange, Heilbron, & Kok, 2018;Yuille & Kersten, 2006;Stokes et al, 2012;Fazio, Ledbetter, & Towles-Schwen 2000;Freeman & Johnson, 2016;Kunda & Sherman-Williams, 1993, Frable & Bem, 1985Bargh, Lombardi, & Higgins, 1988;Hugenberg & Bodenhausen, 2003;Caruso, Mead, & Balcetis, 2009;Proffitt et al, 2003;Wood et al, 2016;Salomon et al, 2013). These representations can be activated by current goals, motivations, emotions, beliefs, and contextual cues (Voss, Rothermund, & Brandtstädter, 2008;Leong et al, 2019;Balcetis & Dunning, 2006;Caruso et al, 2009;Phelps, Ling, & Carrasco, 2006;Barrett & Bar, 2009;Anderson et al, 2011;Pitts, Wilson, & Hugenberg, 2014;Sacco et al, 2011;Van de Cruys, Schouten, & Wagemans, 2013;Levari et al, 2018;Fein, Goethels, & Kugler, 2007;Trope, 1986), but they can also be chronically activated by culture and identity-related processes (Markus, Smith, & Moreland, 1985;Nisbett & Miyamoto, 2005;Golubickis et al, 2020;Xiao, Coppin, & Van Bavel, 2016).…”
Section: Proposition 3: Ceeing Yields Idiosyncratic Subjective Construals As a Results Of The Integration Of Sensory And Non-sensory Inpumentioning
confidence: 99%