1952
DOI: 10.1126/science.115.2978.77
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Partial Reinforcement and Conditioned Heart Rate Response in Human Subjects

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

1
17
0

Year Published

1966
1966
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 4 publications
1
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…At the cardiac level, the aversive CS+ evoked more cardiac deceleration compared with the neutral CS+ after the first acquisition block in Study 1 and (marginally significantly) in Study 2. Because fear-conditioned CSs+ generally evoke cardiac deceleration or “fear bradycardia” (Notterman et al, 1952; Panitz et al, 2015; Sperl et al, 2016), this finding further supports successful imagery-based fear learning from Block 1 to Block 2. In addition, the two nonthreatening stimuli differed from each other; there was a relative acceleration to the neutral CS+ as opposed to the CS–, consistent with imagery tasks evoking cardiac acceleration (Vrana & Lang, 1990).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 56%
“…At the cardiac level, the aversive CS+ evoked more cardiac deceleration compared with the neutral CS+ after the first acquisition block in Study 1 and (marginally significantly) in Study 2. Because fear-conditioned CSs+ generally evoke cardiac deceleration or “fear bradycardia” (Notterman et al, 1952; Panitz et al, 2015; Sperl et al, 2016), this finding further supports successful imagery-based fear learning from Block 1 to Block 2. In addition, the two nonthreatening stimuli differed from each other; there was a relative acceleration to the neutral CS+ as opposed to the CS–, consistent with imagery tasks evoking cardiac acceleration (Vrana & Lang, 1990).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 56%
“…US-alone control. Repeated presentations of the US alone are made in order to control for sensitization by, or habituation to, the US (Notterman, Schoenfeld, & Bersh, 1952;Wickens & Wickens, 1940).…”
Section: Traditional Control Proceduresmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Understanding mechanisms of human fear acquisition, extinction, and their consolidation is crucial to refine current models on emotional learning and improve treatment of anxiety disorders (Hofmann, 2008). Although the cardiac fear response has been an important psychophysiological index in fear conditioning studies for decades (e.g., Bradley, Moulder, & Lang, 2005;De Leon, 1964;L opez, Poy, Pastor, Segarra, & Molt o, 2009;Notterman, 1952), its underlying neural mechanisms are not well understood. However, understanding cardiac activity in fear is important, given that it (a) reflects a broad variety of central and peripheral autonomic influences (Berntson et al, 1997), (b) is crucial for the initiation of different behavioral responses (Berntson, Quigley, & Lozano, 2007), and (c) is an important indicator of psychosomatic health (Thayer, Ahs, Fredrikson, Sollers, & Wager, 2012).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A prominent CR component in response to fear-conditioned CS is the cardiac fear response. This response to nonnoxious threat stimuli typically includes an early (i.e., 2-6 s poststimulus) deceleration of the heart beat both in humans (Bradley et al, 2005;De Leon, 1964;Hugdahl, 1979;L opez et al, 2009;Notterman, 1952; but also see Hodes, Cook, & Lang, 1985) and animals (Applegate, Frysinger, & Kapp, 1982;Burhans, Smith-Bell, & Schreurs, 2010;Holdstock & Schwartzbaum, 1965;Kuniecki, Coenen, & Kaiser, 2002;Pascoe & Kapp, 1985). This deceleration has been commonly interpreted as an index of heightened vigilance and facilitation of sensory input of environmental stimuli (Lang & Bradley, 2010;Lang, Davis, & € Ohman, 2000;L€ ow, Lang, Smith, & Bradley, 2008).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%