1999
DOI: 10.3758/bf03205536
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Parsing silhouettes: The short-cut rule

Abstract: Many researchers have proposed that, for the purpose of recognition, human vision parses shapes into component parts. Precisely how is not yet known. The minima rule for silhouettes (Hoffman & Richards, 1984) defines boundary points at which to parse but does not tell how to use these points to cut silhouettes and, therefore, does not tell what the parts are. In this paper, we propose the short-cut rule, which states that, other things being equal, human vision prefers to use the shortest possible cuts to pars… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
159
0

Year Published

2000
2000
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
3
3
3

Relationship

3
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 158 publications
(165 citation statements)
references
References 57 publications
6
159
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Specifically, although the minima rule provides a number of candidate part boundaries (namely, the negative minima of curvature), it does not indicate how these boundaries should be paired to form part cuts that segment the shape. Furthermore, even in shapes containing exactly two negative minima, simply connecting these two minima does not necessarily yield intuitive part segmentations (see e.g., Singh, Seyranian & Hoffman, 1999;Singh & Hoffman, 2001 for examples). The basic limitation of the minima rule stems from the fact that localizing negative minima of curvature involves only the local geometry of the bounding contour of the shape, but not the nonlocal geometry its interior region (see Section 4 for more on this important distinction).…”
Section: Part-based Representations Of Shapementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically, although the minima rule provides a number of candidate part boundaries (namely, the negative minima of curvature), it does not indicate how these boundaries should be paired to form part cuts that segment the shape. Furthermore, even in shapes containing exactly two negative minima, simply connecting these two minima does not necessarily yield intuitive part segmentations (see e.g., Singh, Seyranian & Hoffman, 1999;Singh & Hoffman, 2001 for examples). The basic limitation of the minima rule stems from the fact that localizing negative minima of curvature involves only the local geometry of the bounding contour of the shape, but not the nonlocal geometry its interior region (see Section 4 for more on this important distinction).…”
Section: Part-based Representations Of Shapementioning
confidence: 99%
“…In this paper, we will study the simple case of two-part shapes that can be segmented by joining two negative minima of curvature on the contour of the shape. In addition to negative minima, recent work has also pointed to the role of more global, regionbased, geometric factors in determining perceived shape in general (Burbeck & Pizer, 1995;Kimia, Tannenbaum, & Zucker, 1995) and part structure in particular (Siddiqi & Kimia, 1995;Siddiqi, Tresness, & Kimia, 1996;Singh & Hoffman, 2001;Singh, Seyranian, & Hoffman, 1999). The method developed in this paper will allow us to study more complex cases as well.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast, views that did not contain any such significant features were invariably difficult to identify. Such convex and concave regions were shown by Koenderink and van Doorn in 1976 to correspond to elliptic (ie`bump-like') and hyperbolic (ie`saddle-like') regions on the surface of a 3-D object, respectively (also see Hoffman and Richards 1984;Koenderink 1984aKoenderink , 1984bRichards et al 1987;Singh et al 1999). Koenderink and van Doorn's finding is important, because all smoothly curved solid objects can be described or represented in terms of these qualitatively distinct types of surface regions, along with the parabolic (ie`cylinderlike') surface regions that separate them.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%