2012
DOI: 10.3758/s13428-012-0245-6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Parsing eye-tracking data of variable quality to provide accurate fixation duration estimates in infants and adults

Abstract: Researchers studying infants’ spontaneous allocation of attention have traditionally relied on hand-coding infants’ direction of gaze from videos; these techniques have low temporal and spatial resolution and are labor intensive. Eye-tracking technology potentially allows for much more precise measurement of how attention is allocated at the subsecond scale, but a number of technical and methodological issues have given rise to caution about the quality and reliability of high temporal resolution data obtained… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
178
0
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 133 publications
(182 citation statements)
references
References 72 publications
(91 reference statements)
2
178
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Eye-tracking data from infants may contain considerably higher levels of noise than data from more compliant participants such as adults due to various factors including their high degree of movement, lack of compliance to the task, poor calibration and corneal reflection disturbances due to the underdeveloped cornea and iris (Hessels, Andersson, Hooge, Nyström, & Kemner, 2015;Saez de Urabain, Johnson, & Smith, 2015;Wass, Smith, & Johnson, 2013). To account for this potential quality/age confound, dedicated in-house software for parsing and cleaning eye tracking data has been developed (GraFix, Saez de Urabain et al, 2015).…”
Section: Fixation Durations (Fds)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Eye-tracking data from infants may contain considerably higher levels of noise than data from more compliant participants such as adults due to various factors including their high degree of movement, lack of compliance to the task, poor calibration and corneal reflection disturbances due to the underdeveloped cornea and iris (Hessels, Andersson, Hooge, Nyström, & Kemner, 2015;Saez de Urabain, Johnson, & Smith, 2015;Wass, Smith, & Johnson, 2013). To account for this potential quality/age confound, dedicated in-house software for parsing and cleaning eye tracking data has been developed (GraFix, Saez de Urabain et al, 2015).…”
Section: Fixation Durations (Fds)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although a number of published empirical articles (Nyström, Andersson, Holmqvist, & van de Weijer, 2013;Wass et al, 2014;Wass, Smith, & Johnson, 2013), methodological reviews (Gredebäck, Johnson, & von Hofsten, 2010;Karatekin, 2007;Oakes, 2012), publication guidelines (e.g., Oakes, 2010), and conferences (e.g., EyeTracKids) have addressed the use of eye-gaze measures to study typical development, methodological issues specific to children with NDD have received less attention (but see Kylliäinen et al, 2014;Sasson & Elison, 2012). Discussing this topic is worthwhile because the issues that arise in studies of individuals with NDD may differ from those that arise in studies of typically developing individuals.…”
Section: Considerations For Using Eye-gaze Methods In Studies Of Nddmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Manual coding is neither automatic nor free of the risk of bias because it relies on human coders to make decisions about gaze location (Oakes, 2012;Wass et al, 2013). In addition, manual coding takes time (e.g., 1-2 hr for a 4-min video) as does training coders to lab standards and coordinating lab-wide agreement checks (see "Manual Eye-Gaze Coding").…”
Section: Determining Gaze Locationmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations