2015
DOI: 10.3758/s13421-015-0511-9
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Parafoveal activation of sign translation previews among deaf readers during the reading of Chinese sentences

Abstract: In the present study, we manipulated the different types of information available in the parafovea during the reading of Chinese sentences and examined whether deaf readers could activate sign translations of Chinese words during reading. The main finding was that, as compared to unrelated previews, the deaf readers had longer fixation durations on the target words when sign-phonologically related preview words were presented; this preview cost effect due to signphonological relatedness was absent for reading-… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

1
13
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 48 publications
1
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Elliott et al (2012) suggested that those phonological units, rather than being based on acoustic information, are based on mouth-shape-based units acquired through lip-reading. Some studies have shown, upon exposure to a word’s written form, deaf readers automatically activate a mental representation of the word’s sign translation (Chiu & Wu, 2016; Meade et al, 2017; Morford et al, 2011; Pan et al, 2015; Thierfelder, Wigglesworth, et al, 2020), a process which appears to involve action simulation of the sign in the brain’s sensorimotor system (Quandt & Kubicek, 2018).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Elliott et al (2012) suggested that those phonological units, rather than being based on acoustic information, are based on mouth-shape-based units acquired through lip-reading. Some studies have shown, upon exposure to a word’s written form, deaf readers automatically activate a mental representation of the word’s sign translation (Chiu & Wu, 2016; Meade et al, 2017; Morford et al, 2011; Pan et al, 2015; Thierfelder, Wigglesworth, et al, 2020), a process which appears to involve action simulation of the sign in the brain’s sensorimotor system (Quandt & Kubicek, 2018).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…On the other hand, DHH signing children have sign-based representations which may support development of word reading skills. Indeed, experimental evidence indicates that there is a signbased route to word reading for DHH signers (Barca, Pezzulo, Castrataro, Rinaldi, & Caselli, 2013;Conlin & Paivio, 1975;Kubus, Villwock, Morford, & Rathman, 2015;Morford, Kroll, Piñar, & Wilkinson, 2014;Morford, Wilkinson, Villwock, Piñar, & Kroll, 2011;Ormel, Hermans, Knoors, & Verhoeven, 2012;Pan, Shu, Wang, & Yan, 2015;Treiman & Hirsh-Pasek, 1983). This is quite remarkable given that sign language and written language do not correspond in sub-lexical structure.…”
Section: Sign Language Skills and Learning To Read Wordsmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…Earlier research on word reading in DHH signing children indicates an important role for sign language skills relating to sub-lexical and lexical processing. In particular, sign language PA has been reported to be correlated with word reading (McQuarrie & Abbott, 2013), and experimental evidence indicates that signs can become automatically associated with their orthographic counterparts (e.g., Morford et al, 2011;Ormel et al, 2012;Pan et al, 2015). In addition, fingerspelling ability, that is, proficiency in accessing representations of and producing the handshapes that correspond to written letters in a manual alphabet, may facilitate establishment of new written vocabulary (Haptonstall-Nykaza & Schick, 2007), and has been found to be related to reading skills (Stone et al, 2015).…”
Section: Sign Language and Word Readingmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations