2010
DOI: 10.1080/02687030903580333
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Overt use of a tactile/kinaesthetic strategy shifts to covert processing in rehabilitation of letter-by-letter reading

Abstract: Background Letter-by-letter readers identify each letter of the word they are reading serially in left to right order before recognizing the word. When their letter naming is also impaired, letter-by-letter reading is inaccurate and can render even single word reading very poor. Tactile and/or kinesthetic strategies have been reported to improve reading in these patients, but only under certain conditions or for a limited set of stimuli. Aims The primary aim of the current study was to determine whether a ta… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
18
0
1

Year Published

2011
2011
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
1
18
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Our proposed interpretation that JW was performing the reading tasks with less effort were consistent with the subjective view of JW’s performance at the end of treatment. From a therapeutic standpoint, these findings are consistent with the rationale of training beyond initial mastery of skills in order to increase automaticity and/or generalization (see, for example, Lott, Carney, Glezer, & Friedman, 2010; Lott & Friedman, 1999). In that vein, the first phase of treatment served to re-establish letter-to-sound and sound-to-letter relations and the application to regularly spelled words and pseudoword spelling.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 79%
“…Our proposed interpretation that JW was performing the reading tasks with less effort were consistent with the subjective view of JW’s performance at the end of treatment. From a therapeutic standpoint, these findings are consistent with the rationale of training beyond initial mastery of skills in order to increase automaticity and/or generalization (see, for example, Lott, Carney, Glezer, & Friedman, 2010; Lott & Friedman, 1999). In that vein, the first phase of treatment served to re-establish letter-to-sound and sound-to-letter relations and the application to regularly spelled words and pseudoword spelling.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 79%
“…Six patients (HT, Tuomainen & Laine, 1991; XX (no initials given), Rothi & Moss, 1992; DM, Arguin & Bub, 1994; VT, Maher, Clayton, Barrett, Schober Peterson, & Rothi, 1998; PA1 and PA2, Lacey, Lott, Snider, Sperling, & Friedman, 2010) are considered pure alexics according to this definition, as they have alexia with no (reported) agraphia or aphasia. Of the remaining patients, one has moderate anomia (Rothi, Greenwald, Maher, & Ochipa, 1998), and another seven are reported to have aphasia (TT, Tuomainen & Laine, 1991; TL, Lott, Friedman, & Linebaugh, 1994; FD, Sage, Hesketh, & Lambon Ralph, 2005; KA, Ablinger & Domahs, 2009; LDR, DBR, IND, Lott, Carney, Glezer, & Friedman, 2010). Eleven patients are reported to have writing deficits consistent with agraphia (TT, Tuomainen & Laine, 1991; KV, Daniel, Bolter, & Long, 1992; TL, Lott et al, 1994; SI, Behrmann & McLeod, 1995; DL, Lott & Friedman, 1999; RS, Friedmann & Lott, 2000; FD, Sage et al, 2005; RB, Beeson, Magloire, & Robey, 2005; LDR, DBR, IND, Lott et al, 2010), while one had quite severe spelling difficulties premorbidly (HL, Beeson, 1998).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There have been many case studies of rehabilitation in pure alexia (Moyer, 1979; Tuomainen and Laine, 1991; Daniel et al , 1992; Arguin and Bub, 1994; Lott et al , 1994, 2010; Behrmann and McLeod, 1995; Seki et al , 1995; Beeson, 1998; Maher et al , 1998; Rothi et al , 1998; Friedman and Lott, 2000; Sage et al , 2005; Ablinger and Domahs, 2009; Lacey et al , 2010); but to date, no group-level studies have been published and there is no generally accepted rehabilitation approach in current clinical use. Attempts to train compensatory reading strategies, such as kinaesthetic reading (tracing the letters to use the intact writing ability to facilitate word recognition), have had positive effects (Lott et al , 1994, 2010; Seki et al , 1995; Maher et al , 1998; Sage et al , 2005).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Attempts to train compensatory reading strategies, such as kinaesthetic reading (tracing the letters to use the intact writing ability to facilitate word recognition), have had positive effects (Lott et al , 1994, 2010; Seki et al , 1995; Maher et al , 1998; Sage et al , 2005). However, the maximum reading speeds achievable using this technique are likely to be limited by the patient’s writing speed, and may actively prevent the recovery of the whole-word recognition strategy that affords rapid and efficient reading in skilled readers (Coslett et al , 1993).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%