2001
DOI: 10.1086/320036
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Overreporting Voting

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
26
0

Year Published

2007
2007
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
2

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 324 publications
(37 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
2
26
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It seems therefore that the higher turnout in the samples is mostly because of the self‐reported nature of this figure, and not so much because of ‘self‐prophecy’ effects in the treatment group. Our finding is thus in line with the results of B ernstein , C hadha and M ontjoy (2001) and T raugott and K atosh (1979), who also find an over‐reporting gap of about 20% points (reported 70%, as opposed to real 50%). Our finding differs from that of G erber and G reen (2001), who do not find an indication that phone calls increase voter turnout.…”
Section: Datasupporting
confidence: 92%
“…It seems therefore that the higher turnout in the samples is mostly because of the self‐reported nature of this figure, and not so much because of ‘self‐prophecy’ effects in the treatment group. Our finding is thus in line with the results of B ernstein , C hadha and M ontjoy (2001) and T raugott and K atosh (1979), who also find an over‐reporting gap of about 20% points (reported 70%, as opposed to real 50%). Our finding differs from that of G erber and G reen (2001), who do not find an indication that phone calls increase voter turnout.…”
Section: Datasupporting
confidence: 92%
“…Using voter lists for the study of turnout has some great virtues, but also limits. The single greatest advantage is that turnout is accurately recorded in the voter list—it is observed behavior and is therefore not plagued by the overreport problem inherent in surveys that ask individuals to self‐report their vote history (Clausen, 1968; Anderson and Silver, 1986; Bernstein, Chadha, and Montjoy, 2001), or by the ecological inference problem inherent in designs that look at aggregate turnout to make inferences about individual‐level behavior (King, 1997). Additionally, since we are computing statistical models on the population of registered voters, we have enough Level‐1 observations to allow for meaningful clustering within communities.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…2) The percentage of people who do not vote is typically underestimated in both pre-election and post-election surveys (e.g., Bernstein, Chadha, & Montjoy, 2001). However, it has been shown that this does not substantially affect the predictive power of the explanatory variables, as comparisons of data that included self-reports and data that were validated using voter records revealed similar results (e.g., Katosh & Traugott, 1981).…”
Section: Background and Hypothesesmentioning
confidence: 99%